Linguistic typology

Linguistic typology (or language typology) is a field of linguistics that studies and classifies languages according to their structural features to allow their comparison. Its aim is to describe and explain the structural diversity and the common properties of the world's languages.[1] Its subdisciplines include, but are not limited to phonological typology, which deals with sound features; syntactic typology, which deals with word order and form; lexical typology, which deals with language vocabulary; and theoretical typology, which aims to explain the universal tendencies.[2]

Linguistic typology is contrasted with genealogical linguistics on the grounds that typology groups languages or their grammatical features based on formal similarities rather than historic descendence.[3] The issue of genealogical relation is however relevant to typology because modern data sets aim to be representative and unbiased. Samples are collected evenly from different language families, emphasizing the importance of lesser-known languages in gaining insight into human language.[4]

History

Speculations of the existence of a (logical) general or universal grammar underlying all languages were published in the Middle Ages, especially by the Modistae school. At the time, Latin was the model language of linguistics, although transcribing Irish and Icelandic into the Latin alphabet was found problematic. The cross-linguistic dimension of linguistics was established in the Renaissance period. For example, Grammaticae quadrilinguis partitiones (1544) by Johannes Drosaeus compared French and the three ‘holy languages’, Hebrew, Greek, and Latin. The approach was expanded by the Port-Royal Grammar (1660) of Antoine Arnauld and Claude Lancelot, who added Spanish, Italian, German and Arabic. Nicolas Beauzée's 1767 book includes examples of English, Swedish, Lappish, Irish, Welsh, Basque, Quechua, and Chinese.[5]

The conquest and conversion of the world by Europeans gave rise to 'missionary linguistics' producing first-hand word lists and grammatical descriptions of exotic languages. Such work is accounted for in the ‘Catalogue of the Languages of the Populations We Know’, 1800, by the Spanish Jesuit Lorenzo Hervás. Johann Christoph Adelung collected the first large language sample with the Lord's prayer in almost five hundred languages (posthumous 1817).[5]

More developed nineteenth-century comparative works include Franz Bopp's 'Conjugation System' (1816) and Wilhelm von Humboldt's ‘On the Difference in Human Linguistic Structure and Its Influence on the Intellectual Development of Mankind’ (posthumous 1836). In 1818, August Wilhelm Schlegel made a classification of the world's languages into three types: (i) languages lacking grammatical structure, e.g. Chinese; (ii) agglutinative languages, e.g. Turkish; and (iii) inflectional languages, which can be synthetic like Latin and Ancient Greek, or analytic like French. This idea was later developed by others including August Schleicher, Heymann Steinthal, Franz Misteli, Franz Nicolaus Finck, and Max Müller.[3]

The word 'typology' was proposed by Georg von der Gabelentz in his Sprachwissenschaft (1891). Louis Hjelmslev proposed typology as a large-scale empirical-analytical endeavour of comparing grammatical features to uncover the essence of language. Such a project began from the 1961 conference on language universals at Dobbs Ferry. Speakers included Roman Jakobson, Charles F. Hockett, and Joseph Greenberg who proposed forty-five different types of linguistic universals based on his data sets from thirty languages. Greenberg's findings were mostly known from the nineteenth-century grammarians, but his systematic presentation of them would serve as a model for modern typology.[3] Winfred P. Lehmann introduced Greenbergian typological theory to Indo-European studies in the 1970s.[6]

During the twentieth century, typology based on missionary linguistics became centered around SIL International, which today hosts its catalogue of living languages, Ethnologue, as an online database. The Greenbergian or universalist approach is accounted for by the World Atlas of Language Structures, among others. Typology is also done within the frameworks of functional grammar including Functional Discourse Grammar, Role and Reference Grammar, and Systemic Functional Linguistics. During the early years of the twenty-first century, however, the existence of linguistic universals became questioned by linguists proposing evolutionary typology.[7]

Method

Quantitative typology deals with the distribution and co-occurrence of structural patterns in the languages of the world.[8] Major types of non-chance distribution include:

  • preferences (for instance, absolute and implicational universals, semantic maps, and hierarchies)
  • correlations (for instance, areal patterns, such as with a Sprachbund)

Linguistic universals are patterns that can be seen cross-linguistically. Universals can either be absolute, meaning that every documented language exhibits this characteristic, or statistical, meaning that this characteristic is seen in most languages or is probable in most languages. Universals, both absolute and statistical can be unrestricted, meaning that they apply to most or all languages without any additional conditions. Conversely, both absolute and statistical universals can be restricted or implicational, meaning that a characteristic will be true on the condition of something else (if Y characteristic is true, then X characteristic is true).[9] An example of an implicational hierarchy is that dual pronouns are only found in languages with plural pronouns while singular pronouns (or unspecified in terms of number) are found in all languages. The implicational hierarchy is thus singular < plural < dual (etc.).

Qualitative typology develops cross-linguistically viable notions or types that provide a framework for the description and comparison of languages.

Subfields

The main subfields of linguistic typology include the empirical fields of syntactic, phonological and lexical typology. Additionally, theoretical typology aims to explain the empirical findings, especially statistical tendencies or implicational hierarchies.

Syntactic typology

Syntactic typology studies a vast array of grammatical phenomena from the languages of the world. Two well-known issues include dominant order and left-right symmetry.

Dominant order

One set of types reflects the basic order of subject, verb, and direct object in sentences:

These labels usually appear abbreviated as "SVO" and so forth, and may be called "typologies" of the languages to which they apply. The most commonly attested word orders are SOV and SVO while the least common orders are those that are object initial with OVS being the least common with only four attested instances.[10]

In the 1980s, linguists began to question the relevance of geographical distribution of different values for various features of linguistic structure. They may have wanted to discover whether a particular grammatical structure found in one language is likewise found in another language in the same geographic location.[11] Some languages split verbs into an auxiliary and an infinitive or participle and put the subject and/or object between them. For instance, German (Ich habe einen Fuchs im Wald gesehen - *"I have a fox in-the woods seen"), Dutch (Hans vermoedde dat Jan Marie zag leren zwemmen - *"Hans suspected that Jan Marie saw to learn to swim") and Welsh (Mae'r gwirio sillafu wedi'i gwblhau - *"Is the checking spelling after its to complete"). In this case, linguists base the typology on the non-analytic tenses (i.e. those sentences in which the verb is not split) or on the position of the auxiliary. German is thus SVO in main clauses and Welsh is VSO (and preposition phrases would go after the infinitive).

Many typologists[who?] classify both German and Dutch as V2 languages, as the verb invariantly occurs as the second element of a full clause.

Some languages allow varying degrees of freedom in their constituent order, posing a problem for their classification within the subject–verb–object schema. Languages with bound case markings for nouns, for example, tend to have more flexible word orders than languages where case is defined by position within a sentence or presence of a preposition. For example, in some languages with bound case markings for nouns, such as Language X, varying degrees of freedom in constituent order are observed. These languages exhibit more flexible word orders, allowing for variations like Subject-Verb-Object (SVO) structure, as in 'The cat ate the mouse,' and Object-Subject-Verb (OSV) structure, as in 'The mouse the cat ate.' To define a basic constituent order type in this case, one generally looks at frequency of different types in declarative affirmative main clauses in pragmatically neutral contexts, preferably with only old referents. Thus, for instance, Russian is widely considered an SVO language, as this is the most frequent constituent order under such conditions—all sorts of variations are possible, though, and occur in texts. In many inflected languages, such as Russian, Latin, and Greek, departures from the default word-orders are permissible but usually imply a shift in focus, an emphasis on the final element, or some special context. In the poetry of these languages, the word order may also shift freely to meet metrical demands. Additionally, freedom of word order may vary within the same language—for example, formal, literary, or archaizing varieties may have different, stricter, or more lenient constituent-order structures than an informal spoken variety of the same language.

On the other hand, when there is no clear preference under the described conditions, the language is considered to have "flexible constituent order" (a type unto itself).

An additional problem is that in languages without living speech communities, such as Latin, Ancient Greek, and Old Church Slavonic, linguists have only written evidence, perhaps written in a poetic, formalizing, or archaic style that mischaracterizes the actual daily use of the language.[12] The daily spoken language of Sophocles or Cicero might have exhibited a different or much more regular syntax than their written legacy indicates.

Theoretical issues

The below table indicates the distribution of the dominant word order pattern of over 5,000 individual languages and 366 language families. SOV is the most common type in both although much more clearly in the data of language families including isolates. 'NODOM' represents languages without a single dominant order.[13]

Type Languages % Families %
SOV 2,275 43.3% 239 65.3%
SVO 2,117 40.3% 55 15%
VSO 503 9.5% 27 7.4%
VOS 174 3.3% 15 4.1%
NODOM 124 2.3% 26 7.1%
OVS 40 0.7% 3 0.8%
OSV 19 0.3% 1 0.3%

Though the reason of dominance is sometimes considered an unsolved or unsolvable typological problem, several explanations for the distribution pattern have been proposed. Evolutionary explanations include those by Thomas Givon (1979), who suggests that all languages stem from an SOV language but are evolving into different kinds; and by Derek Bickerton (1981), who argues that the original language was SVO, which supports simpler grammar employing word order instead of case markers to differentiate between clausal roles.[14]

Universalist explanations include a model by Russell Tomlin (1986) based on three functional principles: (i) animate before inanimate; (ii) theme before comment; and (iii) verb-object bonding. The three-way model roughly predicts the real hierarchy (see table above) assuming no statistical difference between SOV and SVO, and, also, no statistical difference between VOS and OVS. By contrast, the processing efficiency theory of John A. Hawkins (1994) suggests that constituents are ordered from shortest to longest in VO languages, and from longest to shortest in OV languages, giving rise to the attested distribution. This approach relies on the notion that OV languages have heavy subjects, and VO languages have heavy objects, which is disputed.[14]

Left-right symmetry

A second major way of syntactic categorization is by excluding the subject from consideration. It is a well-documented typological feature that languages with a dominant OV order (object before verb), Japanese for example, tend to have postpositions. In contrast, VO languages (verb before object) like English tend to have prepositions as their main adpositional type. Several OV/VO correlations have been uncovered.[15]

Correlation VO languages Example (VO)[16] OV languages Example (OV)
Adposition type prepositions of..., than..., on... postpositions ...of, ...than, ...on
Order of noun and genitive noun before genitive father + of John genitive before noun John-of + father
Order of adjective and standard of comparison adjective before standard taller + than Bob standard before adjective Bob-than + taller
Order of verb and adpositional phrase verb before adpositional phrase slept + on the floor adpositional phrase before verb floor-on + slept
Order of verb and manner adverb verb before manner adverb ran + slowly manner adverb before verb slowly + ran
Order of copula and predicative copula before predicate is + a teacher predicate before copula a teacher + is
Order of auxiliary verb and content verb auxiliary before content verb want + to see Mary content verb before auxiliary Mary see-to + want
Place of adverbial subordinator in clause clause-initial subordinators because + Bob has left clause-final subordinators Bob has left-because
Order of noun and relative clause noun before relative clause movies + that we saw either we-seen + movies or movies we-seen
Theoretical issues

Several processing explanations were proposed in the 1980s and 1990s for the above correlations. They suggest that the brain finds it easier to parse syntactic patterns that are either right or left branching, but not mixed. The most widely held such explanation is John A. Hawkins' parsing efficiency theory, which argues that language is a non-innate adaptation to innate cognitive mechanisms. Typological tendencies are considered as being based on language users' preference for grammars that are organized efficiently, and on their avoidance of word orderings that cause processing difficulty. Hawkins's processing theory predicts the above table but also makes predictions for non-correlation pairs including the order of adjective, demonstrative and numeral in respect with the noun. This theory was based on corpus research and lacks support in psycholinguistic studies.[14]

Some languages exhibit regular "inefficient" patterning. These include the VO languages Chinese, with the adpositional phrase before the verb, and Finnish, which has postpositions. But there are few other profoundly exceptional languages. It is suggested more recently that the left-right orientation is limited to role-marking connectives (adpositions and subordinators), stemming directly from the semantic mapping of the sentence. Since the true correlation pairs in the above table either involve such a connective or, arguably, follow from the canonical order, orientation predicts them without making problematic claims.[17]

Morphosyntactic alignment

Another common classification distinguishes nominative–accusative alignment patterns and ergative–absolutive ones. In a language with cases, the classification depends on whether the subject (S) of an intransitive verb has the same case as the agent (A) or the patient (P) of a transitive verb. If a language has no cases, but the word order is AVP or PVA, then a classification may reflect whether the subject of an intransitive verb appears on the same side as the agent or the patient of the transitive verb. Bickel (2011) has argued that alignment should be seen as a construction-specific property rather than a language-specific property.[18]

Many languages show mixed accusative and ergative behaviour (for example: ergative morphology marking the verb arguments, on top of an accusative syntax). Other languages (called "active languages") have two types of intransitive verbs—some of them ("active verbs") join the subject in the same case as the agent of a transitive verb, and the rest ("stative verbs") join the subject in the same case as the patient[example needed]. Yet other languages behave ergatively only in some contexts (this "split ergativity" is often based on the grammatical person of the arguments or on the tense/aspect of the verb).[19] For example, only some verbs in Georgian behave this way, and, as a rule, only while using the perfective (aorist).

Phonological typology

Linguistic typology also seeks to identify patterns in the structure and distribution of sound systems among the world's languages. This is accomplished by surveying and analyzing the relative frequencies of different phonological properties. Exemplary relative frequencies are given below for certain speech sounds formed by obstructing airflow (obstruents). These relative frequencies show that contrastive voicing commonly occurs with plosives, as in English neat and need, but occurs much more rarely among fricatives, such as the English niece and knees. According to a worldwide sample of 637 languages,[20] 62% have the voicing contrast in stops but only 35% have this in fricatives. In the vast majority of those cases, the absence of voicing contrast occurs because there is a lack of voiced fricatives and because all languages have some form of plosive (occlusive),[21] but there are languages with no fricatives. Below is a chart showing the breakdown of voicing properties among languages in the aforementioned sample.

Fricative Voicing
Plosive Voicing
Yes No Total
Yes 117 218 395 (62%)
No 44 198 242 (38%)
Total 221 (35%) 416 (65%) 637

[20]

Languages worldwide also vary in the number of sounds they use. These languages can go from very small phonemic inventories (Rotokas with six consonants and five vowels) to very large inventories (!Xóõ with 128 consonants and 28 vowels). An interesting phonological observation found with this data is that the larger a consonant inventory a language has, the more likely it is to contain a sound from a defined set of complex consonants (clicks, glottalized consonants, doubly articulated labial-velar stops, lateral fricatives and affricates, uvular and pharyngeal consonants, and dental or alveolar non-sibilant fricatives). Of this list, only about 26% of languages in a survey[20] of over 600 with small inventories (less than 19 consonants) contain a member of this set, while 51% of average languages (19-25) contain at least one member and 69% of large consonant inventories (greater than 25 consonants) contain a member of this set. It is then seen that complex consonants are in proportion to the size of the inventory.

Vowels contain a more modest number of phonemes, with the average being 5–6, which 51% of the languages in the survey have. About a third of the languages have larger than average vowel inventories. Most interesting though is the lack of relationship between consonant inventory size and vowel inventory size. Below is a chart showing this lack of predictability between consonant and vowel inventory sizes in relation to each other.

Consonant Inventory Vowel Quality Inventory
Small Average Large Total
Small 47 153 65 265 (39%)
Average 34 105 98 237 (35%)
Large 34 87 57 178 (26%)
Total 115 (17%) 345 (51%) 220 (32%) 680

[20]

See also

Sources

References

  1. ^ Ferguson, Charles A. (1959). "Diglossia". WORD (Worcester). 15 (2): 325–340. doi:10.1080/00437956.1959.11659702. ISSN 0043-7956. S2CID 239352211 – via Tandfonline-com.
  2. ^ Plungyan, V. A. (2011). Modern linguistic typology. Herald of the Russian Academy of Sciences, 81(2), 101-113. doi:10.1134/S1019331611020158
  3. ^ a b c Graffi, Giorgio (2010). "The Pioneers of Linguistic Typology: From Gabelentz to Greenberg". In Song, Jae Jung (ed.). The Oxford Handbook of Linguistic Typology. Oxford University Press. pp. 25–42. doi:10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199281251.013.0003.
  4. ^ Rijkhoff, Jan (2007). "Linguistic Typology: a short history and some current issues". Tidsskrift for Sprogforskning. 5 (1): 1–18. doi:10.7146/tfs.v5i1.529. Retrieved May 19, 2022.
  5. ^ a b Ramat, Paolo (2010). "The (Early) History of Linguistic Typology". In Song, Jae Jung (ed.). The Oxford Handbook of Linguistic Typology. Oxford University Press. pp. 9–23. doi:10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199281251.013.0002.
  6. ^ Lehmann, Winfred P. (1974). "The Syntactic Framework". Proto-Indo-European Syntax. University of Texas Press. ISBN 9780292733411.[permanent dead link]
  7. ^ Evans, Nicholas; Levinson, Stephen C. (2009). "The myth of language universals: Language diversity and its importance for cognitive science". Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 32 (5): 420–429. doi:10.1017/S0140525X0999094X. hdl:11858/00-001M-0000-0012-C29E-4. PMID 19857320.
  8. ^ Quantitative methods in typology. (2005). In R. Köhler, G. Altmann & R. G. Piotrowski (Eds.), (). Berlin • New York: Walter de Gruyter. doi:10.1515/9783110155785.9.554
  9. ^ Moravcsik, Edith (2013). Introducing Language Typology. Cambridge, London: Cambridge University Press. p. 9. ISBN 9780521193405.
  10. ^ Gell-Mann, Murray; Ruhlen, Merritt (October 18, 2011). "The origin and evolution of word order". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 108 (42): 17290–17295. Bibcode:2011PNAS..10817290G. doi:10.1073/pnas.1113716108. ISSN 0027-8424. PMC 3198322. PMID 21987807.
  11. ^ Comrie, Bernard, et al. “Chapter Introduction.” WALS Online - Chapter Introduction, The World Atlas of Language Structures Online, 2013.
  12. ^ [1] Archived February 11, 2021, at the Wayback Machine Issues in the Linguistic Analysis of a Dead Language, with Particular Reference to Ancient Hebrew, Holmstead, R 2006
  13. ^ Hammarström, Harald (2016). "Linguistic diversity and language evolution". Journal of Language Evolution. 1 (1): 19–29. doi:10.1093/jole/lzw002. hdl:11858/00-001M-0000-0029-2F3E-C. Retrieved May 19, 2022.
  14. ^ a b c Song, Jae Jung (2012). Word Order. Cambridge University Press. ISBN 9781139033930.
  15. ^ Dryer, Matthew S. (1992). "Greenbergian Word Order Correlations" (PDF). Language. 68 (1): 81–138. doi:10.2307/416370. JSTOR 416370. Archived (PDF) from the original on July 1, 2010. Retrieved January 1, 2021.
  16. ^ Dryer, Matthew S. (1992). "Greenbergian Word Order Correlations" (PDF). Language. 68 (1): 81–138. doi:10.2307/416370. JSTOR 416370. Archived (PDF) from the original on July 1, 2010. Retrieved January 1, 2021.
  17. ^ Austin, Patrik (2021). "A semantic and pragmatic explanation of harmony". Acta Linguistica Hafniensia. 54 (1): 1–23. doi:10.1080/03740463.2021.1987685. S2CID 244941417.
  18. ^ Bickel, B. "What is typology? - a short note" (PDF). www.uni-leipzig.de (in German). Retrieved March 6, 2017.
  19. ^ Legate, J. A. (2008). Morphological and abstract case. Linguistic Inquiry, 39(1), 55-101. doi:10.1162/ling.2008.39.1.55
  20. ^ a b c d Song, J.J. (ed.) (2011). The Oxford Handbook of Linguistic Typology. Oxford: Oxford University Press. ISBN 978-0-19-928125-1.
  21. ^ König, W. (ed) dtv Atlas zur deutschen Sprache dtv 1994 (in German)

Bibliography

Read other articles:

Artikel ini tidak memiliki referensi atau sumber tepercaya sehingga isinya tidak bisa dipastikan. Tolong bantu perbaiki artikel ini dengan menambahkan referensi yang layak. Tulisan tanpa sumber dapat dipertanyakan dan dihapus sewaktu-waktu.Cari sumber: Hari Kemerdekaan Singapura – berita · surat kabar · buku · cendekiawan · JSTOR Hari KemerdekaanDirayakan olehSingapuraMaknaMemperingati kemerdekaan Singapura dari Malaysia pada 1965PerayaanParade Ha...

 

Not to be confused with St Ives, Cornwall. Human settlement in EnglandSt IveCornish: Sen IvThe parish church in St Ive Church End, constructed c. 1338St IveLocation within CornwallPopulation2,231 (United Kingdom Census 2011 including Bodway )OS grid referenceSX311672Civil parishSt Ive and PensilvaUnitary authorityCornwallCeremonial countyCornwallRegionSouth WestCountryEnglandSovereign stateUnited KingdomPost townLISKEARDPostcode districtPL14Dialling cod...

 

2017 film by Kamaleshwar Mukherjee CockpitMovie posterDirected byKamaleshwar MukherjeeWritten byKamaleshwar MukherjeeScreenplay byKamaleshwar MukherjeeProduced byGurupada Adhikari & DevStarringDevKoel MallickRukmini MaitraZiaul RoshanCinematographySubhankar BharEdited byRabiranjan MaitraMusic byArindom ChatterjeeProductioncompanyDev Entertainment VenturesDistributed byPiyali Films (India)Jaaz Multimedia (Bangladesh)Release dates 22 September 2017 (2017-09-22) (India) 8&...

 

India and Pakistan have played test series from 1978 to 2006 as part of their long standing rivalry in field hockey. They played eight test series in this span out of which Pakistan won six, India won one and one was drawn. Out of 52 matches played Pakistan won 25, India won 16 and 11 were drawn. The goal aggregation so far is Pakistan 124 - 104 India). In 1998, each country hosted four matches out of which Pakistan won four, India won three and one was drawn.[1] The last was played i...

 

Road in Malaysia Federal Route 217Bukit Jalil Highway(Puchong–Sungai Besi Highway)Route informationLength4.30 km (2.67 mi)Existed1995–presentHistoryCompleted in 1997Major junctionsWest endPuchongMajor intersections Damansara–Puchong Expressway Damansara–Puchong ExpresswayB11 Jalan Puchong–Petaling Jaya East–West Link Expressway Kuala Lumpur–Seremban Expressway Sungai Besi Expressway Sungai Besi ExpresswayEast endSungai Besi LocationCountryMalaysiaPrimary...

 

Spanish footballer Not to be confused with Coke (footballer); Koke (footballer, born 1983); or Keko (footballer, born 1991). In this Spanish name, the first or paternal surname is Resurrección and the second or maternal family name is Merodio. Koke Koke with Atlético Madrid in 2019Personal informationFull name Jorge Resurrección Merodio[1]Date of birth (1992-01-08) 8 January 1992 (age 31)[2]Place of birth Madrid, SpainHeight 1.76 m (5 ft 9 in) ...

 

Steffen Heidrich Steffen Heidrich (1990) Personalia Geburtstag 19. Juli 1967 Geburtsort Erlabrunn, DDR Größe 181 cm Position Mittelfeld Junioren Jahre Station 0000–1980 BSG Messgeräte Beierfeld 1980–1984 FC Karl-Marx-Stadt Herren Jahre Station Spiele (Tore)1 1984–1993 FC Karl-Marx-Stadt/Chemnitzer FC 201 (60) 1993–1998 VfB Leipzig 143 (46) 1998–2001 FC Energie Cottbus 39 (13) 2001–2005 Dynamo Dresden 86 (21) Nationalmannschaft Jahre Auswahl Spiele (Tore) 1982 DDR...

 

العلاقات الرومانية الكندية رومانيا كندا   رومانيا   كندا تعديل مصدري - تعديل   العلاقات الرومانية الكندية هي العلاقات الثنائية التي تجمع بين رومانيا وكندا.[1][2][3][4][5] مقارنة بين البلدين هذه مقارنة عامة ومرجعية للدولتين: وجه المقارنة رومانيا...

 

Main article: North American F-86 Sabre An Australian CA-27 Mk.32 Sabre on display at RAAF Base Wagga The North American F-86 Sabre was a post-war jet fighter that entered service with the United States Air Force in 1949 and was retired from active duty by Bolivia in 1994. F-86s were licence-built in Italy by Fiat Aviazione and in Japan by Mitsubishi Heavy Industries; while variants were produced in Australia as the CAC CA-27 Sabre and in Canada as the Canadair CL-13 Sabre. Argentina Argentin...

 

Franciscan friar and Doctor of the Church (1195–1231) For the Italian film, see Anthony of Padua (film). For others known as Saint Anthony, see Saint Anthony. SaintAnthony of PaduaOFMAnthony of Padua by Francisco de Zurbarán, 1627–1630Doctor of the ChurchBornFernando Martins de Bulhões15 August 1195Lisbon, PortugalDied13 June 1231(1231-06-13) (aged 35)Padua, ItalyVenerated inCatholic ChurchAnglican CommunionBeatified30 May 1232Canonized30 May 1232, Spoleto, Italy by Pope Grego...

 

Ukrainian tennis player Nadiia Kichenok Надія КіченокKichenok at the 2019 Wimbledon ChampionshipsCountry (sports) UkraineResidenceDnipro, UkraineBorn (1992-07-20) 20 July 1992 (age 31)Dnipropetrovsk, UkraineHeight1.72 m (5 ft 8 in)Turned pro2006PlaysRight (two-handed backhand)Prize moneyUS$ 1,489,746SinglesCareer record226–174 (56.5%)Career titles0 WTA, 4 ITFHighest rankingNo. 100 (6 January 2014)Grand Slam singles resultsAustral...

 

Spanish financial services company CaixaBank, S.A.Headquarters in BarcelonaTypeSociedad AnónimaTraded asBMAD: CABKISINES0140609019IndustryFinancial servicesFoundedJanuary 2011; 12 years ago (2011-01) in Barcelona, SpainHeadquartersValencia, Spain (registered office)Madrid, Spain (operative office)Barcelona, Spain (operative office)Key peopleJosé Ignacio Goirigolzarri (Chairman), Gonzalo Gortazar (CEO)ProductsUniversal banking, insurance, investment holdingsRevenu...

 

أتال بيهاري فاجبايي (بالهندية: अटल बिहारी वाजपेयी)‏    مناصب وزير الشؤون الخارجية   في المنصب26 مارس 1977  – 28 يوليو 1979  رئيس وزراء الهند   في المنصب16 مايو 1996  – 1 يونيو 1996  بي في ناراسيمها راو  هاراداناهالي دوده ديفي كودا  رئيس وزراء الهند   ف�...

 

This article relies largely or entirely on a single source. Relevant discussion may be found on the talk page. Please help improve this article by introducing citations to additional sources.Find sources: Canton of Val-de-Saire – news · newspapers · books · scholar · JSTOR (January 2022) The canton of Val-de-Saire is an administrative division of the Manche department, northwestern France. It was created at the French canton reorganisation which came i...

 

Frontera entre Libia y Sudán Mapa del paralelo 20° N, que hace parte de la frontera. Mapa detallado de la frontera y los territorios en disputa hasta 1934. Libia Libia  Sudán Longitud total 381 kmHistoriaCreación 1899Trazado actual 1934[editar datos en Wikidata] La frontera entre Libia y Sudán es el límite que separa a los territorios de Libia y Sudán en el desierto del Sahara. Trazado Al norte, la frontera inicia en la triple frontera que se produce por&#...

 

Еврейское изобразительное искусство — часть международной культуры евреев, сформировавшейся в библейские времена и которая традиционно подразделяется на религиозную и светскую, хотя имеет место и их синтез. Содержание 1 Древнееврейское изобразительное искусство 2 Ев�...

 

This article is about the English football and rugby club. For the Swiss sports club, see Lausanne Football and Cricket Club. Rugby teamLausanneFull nameLausanne Football ClubUnionRugby Football UnionFounded1867[1]Disbanded1881; 142 years ago (1881)[1]LocationPeckham/Dulwich, EnglandGround(s)Peckham, New Cross Gate and later Dulwich Team kit Lausanne was a 19th-century football club that fielded teams playing both the association football and rugby football c...

 

Nacho Monreal Monreal con la maglia dell'Arsenal nel 2015 Nazionalità  Spagna Altezza 180 cm Peso 72 kg Calcio Ruolo Difensore Termine carriera 16 agosto 2022 Carriera Giovanili 2003-2006 Osasuna Squadre di club1 2004-2006 Osasuna B36 (3)2006-2011 Osasuna127 (3)2011-2013 Malaga45 (1)2013-2019 Arsenal187 (7)2019-2022 Real Sociedad55 (3) Nazionale 2004-2005 Spagna U-194 (0)2007-2009 Spagna U-219 (0)2009-2018 Spagna22 (1) Palmarès  Confederations Cup Arg...

 

Peta lama Jepang mengenai Selat Luzon Koordinat: 21°0′N 121°0′E / 21.000°N 121.000°E / 21.000; 121.000 Selat Luzon (bahasa Filipina: Kipot ng Luzon) adalah selat antara Pulau Taiwan dan Luzon, Filipina. Selat tersebut menghubungkan Laut Filipina ke Laut China Selatan di Samudera Pasifik barat.[1][2] Deskripsi Selat Luzon kira-kira memiliki 250 kilometer (160 mi) yang terdapat sejumlah pulau yang dikelompokkan menjadi dua kelompok: Kepulauan...

 

Current denomination of Australian currency Fifty Dollars(Australia)Value$50 Australian dollarsSecurity featuresWindow, WatermarkMaterial usedPolymerYears of printing1995–99, 2003–14, 2016, 2018, 2020–21[1]ObverseDesignDavid UnaiponDesigneremerystudioDesign date15 February 2018ReverseDesignEdith CowanDesigneremerystudioDesign date15 February 2018 The Australian fifty-dollar note is an Australian banknote with a face value of fifty Australian dollars (A$50). Since 1995 it has bee...