Share to: share facebook share twitter share wa share telegram print page

Daydreaming

Daydreaming gentleman in 1912

Daydreaming is a stream of consciousness that detaches from current external tasks when one's attention becomes focused on a more personal and internal direction.

Various names of this phenomenon exist, including mind-wandering, fantasies, and spontaneous thoughts. There are many types of daydreams – however, the most common characteristic to all forms of daydreaming meets the criteria for mild dissociation. In addition, the impacts of the various types of daydreams are not identical. While some are disruptive and deleterious, others may be beneficial to some degree.

The term daydreaming is derived from clinical psychologist Jerome L. Singer, whose research created the foundation for nearly all subsequent modern research. The terminologies assigned by modern researchers brings about challenges centering on identifying the common features of daydreaming and building collective work among researchers.[1]

Characteristics and types of daydreaming

Daydreaming consists of self-generated thoughts comprising three distinct categories: thoughts concerning the future and oneself, reflections on the past and others, and the emotional tone of experiences.[2]

Psychologist Jerome L. Singer established three different types of daydreaming and their characteristics, varying in their cognitive states and emotional experiences. These included positive constructive daydreaming, characterized by constructive engagement, planning, pleasant thoughts, vivid imagery, and curiosity; guilty-dysphoric daydreaming, marked by obsessive, guilt-ridden, and anguished fantasies; and poor attentional control, reflecting difficulty focusing on either internal thoughts or external tasks.

Different daydreaming styles have various effects on certain behaviours, such as creativity.[3]

Functions of daydreaming

Future thinking

Daydreaming can be a useful tool to help keep people mindful of their relevant goals, such as imagining future success of a goal to motivate accomplishing a difficult or uninteresting task.[4]

Creative thinking

This function of daydreaming is associated with increased creativity in individuals.[5] The frequency of daydreaming is the highest during simple tasks.[6] It is hypothesized that daydreaming plays an important role in generating creative problem-solving processes.[7] Studies have found that intentional daydreaming is more effective when focused on creative thought processing, rather than spontaneous or disruptive daydreams.[4]

Attentional cycling

Attentional cycling is an adaptive function of daydreaming through which a person’s attention may cycle through multiple target problems at the same time, helping the individual remain positive. When people have a variety of goals, daydreaming can provide an opportunity for people to alternate across different streams of information and thoughts in a healthy way.[7]

Dishabituation

A change in the daydreaming state can lead to dishabituation, a function that can be beneficial during a learning process as it renews attention and interest in stimuli that have become repetitive. One research identified this effect in learning and showed that learning is more effective with distributed practices over time rather than massed practices all at once.[8] Daydreaming can provide a break to allow thoughts to drift away from intensive learning . When you return, you will be able to focus again with the ability to continue focusing on attention-demanding tasks.[7]

Relief from boredom

When people are performing mundane tasks, daydreaming allows their thoughts to detach from current external tasks to relieve boredom. At the same time, this temporary detachment will not stop external activities completely when they are necessary. As a result, daydreaming can cause the perception that time moves more quickly.[7]

Visualizing social scenarios

Daydreaming can also be used to imagine social situations. Social daydreaming is imagining past social occurrences and future events and conversations.[9] According to research, daydreaming and social cognition have strong overlapping similarities when activated portions of the brain are observed.[10][11] These findings indicate that daydreaming is an extension of the brain's experience of social cognition. This is likely because daydreams are often focused on the mental representations of social events, experiences, and people.

The correlation between social daydreaming and positive social relationships suggests that daydreaming about close others can enhance social well-being,[12] reduce loneliness,[13] and increase relationship satisfaction.[14] Recent studies indicate that social daydreaming serves immediate socio-emotional regulation purposes,[15] particularly in fostering feelings of love and connection, suggesting its adaptive role in achieving goals.

Default mode network

According to several studies, daydreaming appears to be the brain's default setting when no other external task is occupying its attention. A group of regions in the brain called the default mode network is lit up only when the brain is left in a sort of ‘idle’ state. These areas of the brain light up in sequence only when daydreaming.[16][10]

Functional theories

There has yet to be a consensus on how the process of mind wandering occurs.[17] Three theories have been devised to explain the occurrences and reasons behind why people daydream. These theories are the distractibility account, executive-function account, and the decoupling account.[18]

The distractibility account theorizes that distracting stimuli, whether internal or external, reflect a failure to disregard or control distractions in the mind.[19] According to this theory, the brain activity increases in response to an increase in attention to mind-wandering and the mind tends to dwell on task unrelated thoughts (TUT's).[18]

The executive-function account theorizes that the mind fails to correctly process task relevant events. This theory is based on the observation of TUT causes an increase in errors regarding task focused thinking, especially tasks requiring executive control.[17][18]

The decoupling account suggests that attention becomes removed, or decoupled, from perceptual information involving an external task, and couples to an internal process. In this process, TUT is enhanced as internal thoughts are disengaged from surrounding distractions as the participant ‘tunes out’ the surrounding environment.[18][19]

Psychological studies and research

Nighttime dreams

Daydream by Paul César Helleu

Freudian psychology interpreted daydreaming as an expression of the repressed instincts, similarly to those revealing themselves in nighttime dreams. In contrast to nighttime dreams, there seems to be a process of "secondary revision" in fantasies that makes them more lucid, like daydreaming. The state of daydreaming is a kind of liminal state between waking (with the ability to think rationally and logically) and sleeping.[20]

Self-reflection

Daydreaming can also be used to reveal personal aspects about an individual. In an experiment directed by Robert Desoille, subjects were asked to imagine different objects over the course of different rounds. Those who imagined more details and sleek objects often saw themselves as more useful and held the belief that they were more capable of growth. Through the daydream, which  involved many fantastical elements, characteristics such as a fear of men or a desire to subdue a selfish personality trait were often revealed.[21]

Self-focused daydreaming can be positive (i.e. a self-reflection) or negative (i.e. a rumination).[17] This will result in either  increased happiness, anti-depressant thinking, rational planning, creativity, and positivism, or conversely, over-thinking negative experiences from the past, pessimistic views of the future, negative mood-episodes, guilt, fear, and poor attention controls.[22][17]

Everyday use

Eric Klinger's research in the 1980s showed that most daydreams are about ordinary, everyday events and help to remind us of mundane tasks. Klinger's research also showed that over 75% of workers in "boring jobs", such as lifeguards and truck drivers, use vivid daydreams to "ease the boredom" of their routine tasks.[23]

Drawbacks and benefits

Some of the major costs of daydreaming summarized by the review are worse performances with reading, sustained attention, mood etc.[7]

The negative consequences of daydreaming on reading performance have been studied the most thoroughly. Research shows that there is a negative correlation between daydreaming frequency and reading comprehension performance, specifically worsened item-specific comprehension and model-building ability.[7]

Disruptive daydreams or spontaneous daydreaming is also characteristic of people with attention-deficit hyperactive disorder (ADHD).[24][25]

Negative mood is another association of daydreaming. Research finds people generally report lower happiness when they are daydreaming than when they are not. For those experiencing positive daydreaming, the same happiness rating is reported between current tasks and pleasant things they are more likely to daydream about. This finding remains true across all activities.[7]

In the late 19th century, Toni Nelson argued that some daydreams with grandiose fantasies are self-gratifying attempts at "wish fulfillment". In the 1950s, some educational psychologists warned parents not to let their children daydream, for fear that the children may be sucked into "neurosis and even psychosis".[23]

While the cost of daydreaming is more thoroughly discussed, the associated benefit is understudied. One potential reason is the payoff of daydreaming is usually private and hidden compared to the measurable cost from external goal-directed tasks. It is hard to know and record people's private thoughts such as personal goals and dreams, so whether daydreaming supports these thoughts is difficult to discuss.[1]

Select research has argued that the mind is not idle during daydreaming, though it is at rest when not attentively engaging in external tasks. Rather, during this process, people indulge themselves in and reflect on fantasies, memories, future goals and psychological selves while still being able to control enough attention to keep easy tasks going and monitor the external environment. Thus, the potential benefits are the skills of internal reflection developed in daydreaming to connect emotional implication of daily life experience with personal meaning building process.[26]

Despite the detrimental impact of daydreaming on aptitude tests which most educational institutions put heavy emphasis on, scholars argue that it is important for children to get internal reflection skills from daydreaming. Research shows that children equipped with these skills have higher academic ability and are socially and emotionally better off.[26]

Besides believing that daydreaming is motivated by unconscious drives and desires, Sigmund Freud[27] also acknowledges that daydreaming can become excessive or pathological in some cases. Such instances can manifest as hysteria, neurosis, and psychopathology. When daydreaming becomes too detached from reality or interferes with everyday functioning, it may be indicative of deeper psychological issues or neurotic conflicts. While Freud didn’t explicitly correlate daydreaming to mental illness, he suggests that certain types of daydreams reflect underlying psychological disturbances.  

Various studies have also focused on maladaptive daydreaming, which describes vivid and elaborate daydreams for prolonged periods of time.[28] Individuals who are affected by maladaptive daydreaming often neglect their real-life relationships and obligations, leading to clinical distress and impaired functioning. According to research[29] the most common comorbidities associated with maladaptive daydreaming include attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, anxiety disorders, major depressive disorder, and obsessive-compulsive disorder.

Criticism and Limitations

Research on daydreaming faces challenges due to the difficulty in observing and measuring it compared to other mental tasks.[30] Instead of making broad conclusions about its benefits or drawbacks, researchers should focus on how the content and form of daydreams relate to specific adaptive outcomes. This involves using intensive longitudinal methods to track daydreams in real-world settings and linking them to measurable goals. Integration with social psychological theory can help understand how social daydreams impact social interactions and goal achievement. Combining neuroimaging studies with experience-sampling studies can offer insights into the neural mechanisms underlying the effects of daydreaming on social navigation.[31]

See also

References

  1. ^ a b Singer, Jerome L.; Kaufman, Scott Barry; McMillan, Rebecca (2013). "Ode to positive constructive daydreaming". Frontiers in Psychology. 4: 626. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00626. ISSN 1664-1078. PMC 3779797. PMID 24065936.
  2. ^ Poerio, Giulia Lara; Smallwood, Jonathan (November 2016). "Daydreaming to navigate the social world: What we know, what we don't know, and why it matters". Social and Personality Psychology Compass. 10 (11): 605–618. doi:10.1111/spc3.12288. ISSN 1751-9004.
  3. ^ Zedelius, Claire M.; Schooler, Jonathan W. (2016). "The Richness of Inner Experience: Relating Styles of Daydreaming to Creative Processes". Frontiers in Psychology. 6: 2063. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.02063. ISSN 1664-1078. PMC 4735674. PMID 26869943.
  4. ^ a b [1](registration required)
  5. ^ Baird, Benjamin; Smallwood, Jonathan; Mrazek, Michael D.; Kam, Julia W. Y.; Franklin, Michael S.; Schooler, Jonathan W. (2012-10-01). "Inspired by distraction: mind wandering facilitates creative incubation". Psychological Science. 23 (10): 1117–1122. doi:10.1177/0956797612446024. ISSN 1467-9280. PMID 22941876. S2CID 46281941.
  6. ^ Smallwood, Jonathan; Schooler, Jonathan W. (November 2006). "The restless mind". Psychological Bulletin. 132 (6): 946–958. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.132.6.946. ISSN 1939-1455. PMID 17073528. S2CID 18882553.
  7. ^ a b c d e f g Mooneyham, Benjamin W.; Schooler, Jonathan W. (March 2013). "The costs and benefits of mind-wandering: a review". Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology. 67 (1): 11–18. doi:10.1037/a0031569. ISSN 1878-7290. PMID 23458547.
  8. ^ Underwood, Benton J.; Ekstrand, Bruce R. (1967). "Effect of distributed practice on paired-associate learning". Journal of Experimental Psychology. 73 (4, Pt. 2): 1–21. doi:10.1037/h0024341. ISSN 0022-1015.
  9. ^ Honeycutt, James M.; Vickery, Andrea J.; Hatcher, Laura C. (2015-04-03). "The Daily Use of Imagined Interaction Features". Communication Monographs. 82 (2): 201–23. doi:10.1080/03637751.2014.953965. ISSN 0363-7751. S2CID 143513182.
  10. ^ a b Poerio, Giulia Lara; Smallwood, Jonathan (2016). "Daydreaming to navigate the social world: What we know, what we don't know, and why it matters" (PDF). Social and Personality Psychology Compass. 10 (11): 605–618. doi:10.1111/spc3.12288.
  11. ^ [2](registration required)
  12. ^ Mar, Raymond A.; Mason, Malia F.; Litvack, Aubrey (March 2012). "How daydreaming relates to life satisfaction, loneliness, and social support: The importance of gender and daydream content". Consciousness and Cognition. 21 (1): 401–407. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2011.08.001. ISSN 1053-8100. PMID 22033437.
  13. ^ Honeycutt, James M.; Edwards, Renee; Zagacki, Kenneth S. (September 1989). "Using Imagined Interaction Features to Predict Measures of Self-Awareness: Loneliness, Locus of Control, Self-Dominance, and Emotional Intensity". Imagination, Cognition and Personality. 9 (1): 17–31. doi:10.2190/02L8-1GMP-JV5C-JQ7X. ISSN 0276-2366.
  14. ^ Honeycutt, James M.; Keaton, Shaughan A. (September 2012). "Imagined Interactions and Personality Preferences as Predictors of Relationship Quality". Imagination, Cognition and Personality. 32 (1): 3–21. doi:10.2190/IC.32.1.b. ISSN 0276-2366.
  15. ^ Poerio, Giulia L.; Totterdell, Peter; Emerson, Lisa-Marie; Miles, Eleanor (May 2015). "Love is the triumph of the imagination: Daydreams about significant others are associated with increased happiness, love and connection". Consciousness and Cognition. 33: 135–144. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2014.12.011. hdl:10072/352183. ISSN 1053-8100. PMID 25584779.
  16. ^ Raichle, M. E.; MacLeod, A. M.; Snyder, A. Z.; Powers, W. J.; Gusnard, D. A.; Shulman, G. L. (2001-01-16). "A default mode of brain function". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 98 (2): 676–682. doi:10.1073/pnas.98.2.676. ISSN 0027-8424. PMC 14647. PMID 11209064.
  17. ^ a b c d Shrimpton, Daisy; McGann, Deborah; Riby, Leigh M. (2017-11-30). "Daydream Believer: Rumination, Self-Reflection and the Temporal Focus of Mind Wandering Content". Europe's Journal of Psychology. 13 (4): 794–809. doi:10.5964/ejop.v13i4.1425. ISSN 1841-0413. PMC 5763464. PMID 29358989.
  18. ^ a b c d Barron, Evelyn; Riby, Leigh M.; Greer, Joanna; Smallwood, Jonathan (2011-05-01). "Absorbed in Thought: The Effect of Mind Wandering on the Processing of Relevant and Irrelevant Events". Psychological Science. 22 (5): 596–601. doi:10.1177/0956797611404083. ISSN 0956-7976. PMID 21460338. S2CID 9341870.
  19. ^ a b Smallwood, Jonathan; Davies, John B.; Heim, Derek; Finnigan, Frances; Sudberry, Megan; O'Connor, Rory; Obonsawin, Marc (2004-12-01). "Subjective experience and the attentional lapse: Task engagement and disengagement during sustained attention". Consciousness and Cognition. 13 (4): 657–690. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2004.06.003. ISSN 1053-8100. PMID 15522626. S2CID 2514220.
  20. ^ Strachey, J. (1953). The Standard Edition of the Complete Psychological Works of Sigmund Freud, Volume V (1900–1901): The Interpretation of Dreams (Second Part) and On Dreams. London: The Hogarth Press and the Institute of Psycho-analysis. p. 492.
  21. ^ Rocca, R. E. (September 1981). "[Directed daydreaming of Robert Desoille]". Acta Psiquiatrica y Psicologica de America Latina. 27 (4–5): 295–303. ISSN 0001-6896. PMID 7348086.
  22. ^ Lavender, Anna; Watkins, Edward (2004). "Rumination and future thinking in depression". British Journal of Clinical Psychology. 43 (2): 129–142. doi:10.1348/014466504323088015. ISSN 2044-8260. PMID 15169614.
  23. ^ a b Klinger, Eric (October 1987). Psychology Today.
  24. ^ [3](registration required)
  25. ^ [4](registration required)
  26. ^ a b Immordino-Yang, Mary Helen; Christodoulou, Joanna A.; Singh, Vanessa (July 2012). "Rest Is Not Idleness: Implications of the Brain's Default Mode for Human Development and Education". Perspectives on Psychological Science: A Journal of the Association for Psychological Science. 7 (4): 352–364. doi:10.1177/1745691612447308. ISSN 1745-6916. PMID 26168472. S2CID 11957498.
  27. ^ Freud, S. (1908). Creative writers and day dreaming. In P. E. Vernon (Ed.), Creativity). England: Penguin Books.
  28. ^ Soffer-Dudek, Nirit; Somer, Eli (2018). "Trapped in a Daydream: Daily Elevations in Maladaptive Daydreaming Are Associated With Daily Psychopathological Symptoms". Frontiers in Psychiatry. 9: 194. doi:10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00194. ISSN 1664-0640. PMC 5962718. PMID 29867613.
  29. ^ Somer, Eli; Soffer-Dudek, Nirit; Ross, Colin A. (July 2017). "The Comorbidity of Daydreaming Disorder (Maladaptive Daydreaming)". Journal of Nervous & Mental Disease. 205 (7): 525–530. doi:10.1097/NMD.0000000000000685. ISSN 1539-736X. PMID 28598955.
  30. ^ Smallwood, Jonathan (May 2013). "Distinguishing how from why the mind wanders: A process–occurrence framework for self-generated mental activity". Psychological Bulletin. 139 (3): 519–535. doi:10.1037/a0030010. ISSN 1939-1455. PMID 23607430.
  31. ^ Poerio, Giulia Lara; Smallwood, Jonathan (November 2016). "Daydreaming to navigate the social world: What we know, what we don't know, and why it matters". Social and Personality Psychology Compass. 10 (11): 605–618. doi:10.1111/spc3.12288. ISSN 1751-9004.

Read other articles:

Para la población homónima de la provincia de Pontevedra, véase Magán (Pontevedra). Magán municipio de EspañaBanderaEscudo Casa consistorial de Magán MagánUbicación de Magán en España. MagánUbicación de Magán en la provincia de Toledo.País  España• Com. autónoma  Castilla-La Mancha• Provincia  Toledo• Comarca La Sagra• Partido judicial Toledo[1]​Ubicación 39°57′36″N 3°55′55″O / …

?ТроодонтидиЧас існування: пізня юра - крейдовий період163–66 млн р. т. PreꞒ Ꞓ O S D C P T J K Ꝑ N Zanabazar — реконструкція Біологічна класифікація Домен: Ядерні (Eukaryota) Царство: Тварини (Animalia) Тип: Хордові (Chordata) Клас: Завропсиди (Sauropsida) Надряд: Динозаври (Dinosauria) Ряд: Ящеротазові (Sauri…

Наступ на північному заході Сирії (з 2019) Громадянська війна в Сирії Ситуація на північному заході Сирії. Зображені розташування турецьких, російських та іранських спостережних пунктів.(Коричнева лінія — межа за Сочинськими угодами)      Територія під контроле…

هذه المقالة يتيمة إذ تصل إليها مقالات أخرى قليلة جدًا. فضلًا، ساعد بإضافة وصلة إليها في مقالات متعلقة بها. (أبريل 2019) دوغلاس جي. ستيوارت معلومات شخصية الميلاد 5 أكتوبر 1931  تاريخ الوفاة 6 أبريل 2019 (87 سنة) [1]  مواطنة الولايات المتحدة  الحياة العملية المهنة عالم أعصاب …

يفتقر محتوى هذه المقالة إلى الاستشهاد بمصادر. فضلاً، ساهم في تطوير هذه المقالة من خلال إضافة مصادر موثوق بها. أي معلومات غير موثقة يمكن التشكيك بها وإزالتها. (ديسمبر 2018) قرية صيمارة  - قرية -  تقسيم إداري البلد  اليمن المحافظة محافظة سقطرى المديرية مديرية حديبو ال

Tribe of moths Lymantriini imago and caterpillar of Lymantria monacha, Lymantriini Scientific classification Domain: Eukaryota Kingdom: Animalia Phylum: Arthropoda Class: Insecta Order: Lepidoptera Superfamily: Noctuoidea Family: Erebidae Subfamily: Lymantriinae Tribe: LymantriiniHampson, 1893 Lymantriini (sometimes misspelled as Lymantrini) is a tribe of moths of the family Erebidae. This tribe is a group of polyphagous moths that reside mostly in the tropical regions of Afro-Eurasia but also N…

American soldier and politician (1826–1886) John Alexander Logan redirects here. For other uses, see John Logan (disambiguation). John LoganLogan, c. 1880sUnited States Senatorfrom IllinoisIn officeMarch 4, 1879 – December 26, 1886Preceded byRichard OglesbySucceeded byCharles B. FarwellIn officeMarch 4, 1871 – March 3, 1877Preceded byRichard YatesSucceeded byDavid DavisMember of the U.S. House of Representativesfrom Illinois's at-large districtIn officeMarch 4…

American professional wrestler This biography of a living person needs additional citations for verification. Please help by adding reliable sources. Contentious material about living persons that is unsourced or poorly sourced must be removed immediately from the article and its talk page, especially if potentially libelous.Find sources: Mosh wrestler – news · newspapers · books · scholar · JSTOR (April 2010) (Learn how and when to remove this templ…

Colors that represent a nation Part of a series onNationalism Nation forming Nationalism in the Middle Ages Anthem Church Colours Emblem Father Flag Epic God Identity Language Myth Sport State Symbol Treasure Core values Allegiance Independence Patriotism Self-determination Solidarity Types African Anarchist Blind Bourgeois Business Welfare Civic American French Irish Communist Constitutional patriotism Corporate Cyber- Ecological Economic Ethnic Ethnopluralism European Expansionist Integral Lef…

Перуанская коммунистическая партияисп. Partido Comunista Peruano Лидер Роберто де ла Крус Уаман Основатель Хосе Карлос Мариатеги Основана 7 октября 1928 года Штаб-квартира Лима Страна  Перу Идеология марксизм-ленинизм Интернационал Международная встреча коммунистических и рабо…

Konflik Lebanon 1860Abdelkader El Djezairi melindungi penganut Kristen pada saat pertikaian Druze–Kristen 1860, karya Jean Baptiste HuysmansTanggal1860LokasiGunung Lebanon, Lembah Beqaa dan Damaskus, Kekaisaran UtsmaniyahHasil Awalnya kemenangan militer Druze Pembantaian umat Kristen di Gunung Lebanon dan Damaskus Intervensi militer PrancisPihak terlibat Militan pedesaan Maronit Militan Zahalni Dinasti Shihab Klan pedesaan Druze Klan Abu Nakad Klan Imad Klan Talhuq Klan Jumblatt Didukung oleh …

American distance runner For his son, the 1500 metres gold medallist in 2016, see Matthew Centrowitz, Jr. Matthew Centrowitz Sr.Personal informationNationalityAmericanBorn (1955-01-28) January 28, 1955 (age 68)New York, New YorkAlma materUniversity of OregonSportCountryUnited States of AmericaSportTrack and FieldEvent5,000 metersCoached byBill Dellinger Medal record Men’s Athletics Representing  United States Pan American Games 1979 San Juan, PR 5000 m USA Outdoor Championships …

北京青年类型都市情感原作常琳《北京青年》编剧常琳、孙建业导演赵宝刚、王迎主演李晨、任重、杜淳、贺刚制作国家/地区 中国语言汉语普通话集数36集每集长度45分钟制作制作公司完美世界影视播出信息 首播频道浙江卫视、东方卫视、天津卫视、北京卫视播出国家/地区 中国播出日期2012年8月16日— 相关节目相关节目为《奋斗》、《我的青春谁做主》外部链接IMDb…

This article relies largely or entirely on a single source. Relevant discussion may be found on the talk page. Please help improve this article by introducing citations to additional sources.Find sources: Macarena Non Stop – news · newspapers · books · scholar · JSTOR (March 2018) 1996 remix album by Los del RíoMacarena Non StopRemix album by Los del RíoReleasedDecember 21, 1996RecordedJanuary–November 1996GenreLatin popLatin danceLabelBMG US …

For the Sogdian dynasty, see Ikhshids of Sogdia.For the Turkic dynasty in Egypt, see Ikhshidid dynasty. Ikhshid (Sogdian: xšyδ, xšēδ) was the princely title of the Iranian rulers of Soghdia and the Ferghana Valley in Transoxiana during the pre-Islamic and early Islamic periods.[1] The title is of Iranian origin; scholars have derived it variously from the Old Iranian root khshaeta, shining, brilliant, or from khshāyathiya, ruler, king (which is also the origin of the title shah).&#…

American band This article is about the 1980s American band. For the Canadian band, see Little Girls (band). For other uses, see Little Girl. This article relies largely or entirely on a single source. Relevant discussion may be found on the talk page. Please help improve this article by introducing citations to additional sources.Find sources: The Little Girls – news · newspapers · books · scholar · JSTOR (October 2020) The Little GirlsOriginSan Fernando…

Book by Anthony Horowitz This article is about the 2000 novel. For other uses, see Stormbreaker (disambiguation). Stormbreaker First edition (UK)AuthorAnthony HorowitzCountryUnited KingdomLanguageEnglishSeriesAlex Rider seriesGenreAdventure, spy, thrillerPublisherWalker Books (UK)Puffin Books (US)Publication date4 September 2000 (UK)21 May 2001 (US)Media typePrint (hardback and paperback)Pages240 (first edition, paperback)ISBN0-7445-5943-X (first edition, paperback)OCLC44562574Followed …

Japanese manga series For other uses, see Case file (disambiguation). The Kindaichi Case FilesThe Kindaichi Case Files first tankōbon volume cover, featuring Hajime Kindaichi金田一少年の事件簿(Kindaichi Shōnen no Jikenbo)GenreMystery[1] MangaWritten byYōzaburō Kanari (File and Case series)Seimaru Amagi (other series)Illustrated byFumiya SatōPublished byKodanshaEnglish publisherNA: TokyopopImprintShōnen Magazine Comics (1992–2017)Evening KC (2018–)Magazin…

コンチネンタル航空 1713便 事故機の残骸事故の概要日付 1987年11月15日概要 着氷及びパイロットエラーによる離陸失敗[1]現場 アメリカ合衆国・デンバー-ステープルトン国際空港乗客数 77乗員数 5負傷者数 52死者数 28生存者数 54機種 ダグラス DC-9-14運用者 コンチネンタル航空機体記号 N626TX出発地 デンバー-ステープルトン国際空港目的地 ボイシ空港テンプレートを表…

UK-related events during the year of 2012 2012 in the United Kingdom Other years 2010 | 2011 | 2012 (2012) | 2013 | 2014 Countries of the United Kingdom England | Northern Ireland | Scotland | Wales Popular culture Football: England | Scotland | Wales 2012 British Grand Prix 2012 English cricket season 2012 in British television 2012 in British music 2012 in British radio UK in the Eurovision Song Contest 2012 Events from the year 2012 in the United Kingdom. This was the year of the Summer …

Kembali kehalaman sebelumnya