In simple terms, risk is the possibility of something bad happening.[1] Risk involves uncertainty about the effects/implications of an activity with respect to something that humans value (such as health, well-being, wealth, property or the environment), often focusing on negative, undesirable consequences.[2] Many different definitions have been proposed. One international standard definition of risk is the "effect of uncertainty on objectives".[3]
The understanding of risk, the methods of assessment and management, the descriptions of risk and even the definitions of risk differ in different practice areas (business, economics, environment, finance, information technology, health, insurance, safety, security etc). This article provides links to more detailed articles on these areas. The international standard for risk management, ISO 31000, provides principles and general guidelines on managing risks faced by organizations.[4]
Definitions of risk
Oxford English Dictionary
The Oxford English Dictionary (OED) cites the earliest use of the word in English (in the spelling of risque from its French original, 'risque') as of 1621, and the spelling as risk from 1655. While including several other definitions, the OED 3rd edition defines risk as:
(Exposure to) the possibility of loss, injury, or other adverse or
unwelcome circumstance; a chance or situation involving such a possibility.[5]
The International Organization for Standardization (ISO) 31073 provides basic vocabulary to develop common understanding on risk management concepts and terms across different applications. ISO 31073 defines risk as:[6]
Note 1: An effect is a deviation from the expected. It can be positive, negative or both, and can address, create or result in opportunities and threats.[9]
Note 2: Objectives can have different aspects and categories, and can be applied at different levels.
Note 3: Risk is usually expressed in terms of risk sources, potential events, their consequences and their likelihood.
This definition was developed by an international committee representing over 30 countries and is based on the input of several thousand subject-matter experts. It was first adopted in 2002 for use in standards.[10] Its complexity reflects the difficulty of satisfying fields that use the term risk, in different ways. Some restrict the term to negative impacts ("downside risks"), while others also include positive impacts ("upside risks").
Other
"Source of harm". The earliest use of the word "risk" was as a synonym for the much older word "hazard", meaning a potential source of harm. This definition comes from Blount's "Glossographia" (1661)[11] and was the main definition in the OED 1st (1914) and 2nd (1989) editions. Modern equivalents refer to "unwanted events"[12] or "something bad that might happen".[1]
"Chance of harm". This definition comes from Johnson's "Dictionary of the English Language" (1755), and has been widely paraphrased, including "possibility of loss"[5] or "probability of unwanted events".[12]
"Uncertainty about loss". This definition comes from Willett's "Economic Theory of Risk and Insurance" (1901).[13] This links "risk" to "uncertainty", which is a broader term than chance or probability.
"Measurable uncertainty". This definition comes from Knight's "Risk, Uncertainty and Profit" (1921).[14] It allows "risk" to be used equally for positive and negative outcomes. In insurance, risk involves situations with unknown outcomes but known probability distributions.[15]
"Volatility of return". Equivalence between risk and variance of return was first identified in Markovitz's "Portfolio Selection" (1952).[16] In finance, volatility of return is often equated to risk.[17]
"Statistically expected loss". The expected value of loss was used to define risk by Wald (1939) in what is now known as decision theory.[18] The probability of an event multiplied by its magnitude was proposed as a definition of risk for the planning of the Delta Works in 1953, a flood protection program in the Netherlands.[19] It was adopted by the US Nuclear Regulatory Commission (1975),[20] and remains widely used.[12]
"Likelihood and severity of events". The "triplet" definition of risk as "scenarios, probabilities and consequences" was proposed by Kaplan & Garrick (1981).[21] Many definitions refer to the likelihood/probability of events/effects/losses of different severity/consequence, e.g. ISO Guide 73 Note 4.[3]
"Consequences and associated uncertainty". This was proposed by Kaplan & Garrick (1981).[21] This definition is preferred in Bayesian analysis, which sees risk as the combination of events and uncertainties about them.[22]
"Uncertain events affecting objectives". This definition was adopted by the Association for Project Management (1997).[23][24] With slight rewording it became the definition in ISO Guide 73.[3]
"Uncertainty of outcome". This definition was adopted by the UK Cabinet Office (2002)[25] to encourage innovation to improve public services. It allowed "risk" to describe either "positive opportunity or negative threat of actions and events".
"Asset, threat and vulnerability". This definition comes from the Threat Analysis Group (2010) in the context of computer security.[26]
"Human interaction with uncertainty". This definition comes from Cline (2015)[27] in the context of adventure education.
"Potential returns from an event ['a thing that happens or takes place'], where the returns are any changes, effects, consequences, and so on, of the event". This definition from Newsome (2014) expands the neutrality of 'risk' akin to the UK Cabinet Office (2002) and Knight (1921).[28]
Some resolve these differences by arguing that the definition of risk is subjective. For example:
No definition is advanced as the correct one, because there is no one definition that is suitable for all problems. Rather, the choice of definition is a political one, expressing someone's views regarding the importance of different adverse effects in a particular situation.[29]
The Society for Risk Analysis concludes that "experience has shown that to agree on one unified set of definitions is not realistic". The solution is "to allow for different perspectives on fundamental concepts and make a distinction between overall qualitative definitions and their associated measurements."[2]
Practice areas
The understanding of risk, the common methods of management, the measurements of risk and even the definition of risk differ in different practice areas. This section provides links to more detailed articles on these areas.
Business risks arise from uncertainty about the profit of a commercial business[30] due to unwanted events such as changes in tastes, changing preferences of consumers, strikes, increased competition, changes in government policy, obsolescence etc.
Business risks are controlled using techniques of risk management. In many cases they may be managed by intuitive steps to prevent or mitigate risks, by following regulations or standards of good practice, or by insurance. Enterprise risk management includes the methods and processes used by organizations to manage risks and seize opportunities related to the achievement of their objectives.
Economic risk
Economics is concerned with the production, distribution and consumption of goods and services. Economic risk arises from uncertainty about economic outcomes. For example, economic risk may be the chance that macroeconomic conditions like exchange rates, government regulation, or political stability will affect an investment or a company's prospects.[31]
In economics, as in finance, risk is often defined as quantifiable uncertainty about gains and losses.
In finance, risk is the possibility that the actual return on an investment will be different from its expected return.[35] This includes not only "downside risk" (returns below expectations, including the possibility of losing some or all of the original investment) but also "upside risk" (returns that exceed expectations). In Knight's definition, risk is often defined as quantifiable uncertainty about gains and losses. This contrasts with Knightian uncertainty, which cannot be quantified.
Epidemiology is the study and analysis of the distribution, patterns and determinants of health and disease. It is a cornerstone of public health, and shapes policy decisions by identifying risk factors for disease and targets for preventive healthcare.
In the context of public health, risk assessment is the process of characterizing the nature and likelihood of a harmful effect to individuals or populations from certain human activities. Health risk assessment can be mostly qualitative or can include statistical estimates of probabilities for specific populations.
A health risk assessment (also referred to as a health risk appraisal and health & well-being assessment) is a questionnaire screening tool, used to provide individuals with an evaluation of their health risks and quality of life.
Health, safety, and environment risks
Health, safety, and environment (HSE) are separate practice areas; however, they are often linked. The reason is typically to do with organizational management structures; however, there are strong links among these disciplines. One of the strongest links is that a single risk event may have impacts in all three areas, albeit over differing timescales. For example, the uncontrolled release of radiation or a toxic chemical may have immediate short-term safety consequences, more protracted health impacts, and much longer-term environmental impacts. Events such as Chernobyl, for example, caused immediate deaths, and in the longer term, deaths from cancers, and left a lasting environmental impact leading to birth defects, impacts on wildlife, etc.
Information technology (IT) is the use of computers to store, retrieve, transmit, and manipulate data. IT risk (or cyber risk) arises from the potential that a threat may exploit a vulnerability to breach security and cause harm. IT risk management applies risk management methods to IT to manage IT risks. Computer security is the protection of IT systems by managing IT risks.
Information security is the practice of protecting information by mitigating information risks. While IT risk is narrowly focused on computer security, information risks extend to other forms of information (paper, microfilm).
Insurance risk
Insurance is a risk treatment option which involves risk sharing. It can be considered as a form of contingent capital and is akin to purchasing an option in which the buyer pays a small premium to be protected from a potential large loss.
Insurance risk is often taken by insurance companies, who then bear a pool of risks including market risk, credit risk, operational risk, interest rate risk, mortality risk, longevity risks, etc.[37]
The term "risk" has a long history in insurance and has acquired several specialised definitions, including "the subject-matter of an insurance contract", "an insured peril" as well as the more common "possibility of an event occurring which causes injury or loss".[38]
The Occupational Health and Safety Assessment Series (OHSAS) standard OHSAS 18001 in 1999 defined risk as the "combination of the likelihood and consequence(s) of a specified hazardous event occurring". In 2018 this was replaced by ISO 45001 "Occupational health and safety management systems", which use the ISO Guide 73 definition.
Project risk
A project is an individual or collaborative undertaking planned to achieve a specific aim. Project risk is defined as, "an uncertain event or condition that, if it occurs, has a positive or negative effect on a project's objectives". Project risk management aims to increase the likelihood and impact of positive events and decrease the likelihood and impact of negative events in the project.[39][40]
Safety risk
Safety is concerned with a variety of hazards that may result in accidents causing harm to people, property and the environment. In the safety field, risk is typically defined as the "likelihood and severity of hazardous events". Safety risks are controlled using techniques of risk management.
A high reliability organisation (HRO) involves complex operations in environments where catastrophic accidents could occur. Examples include aircraft carriers, air traffic control, aerospace and nuclear power stations. Some HROs manage risk in a highly quantified way. The technique is usually referred to as probabilistic risk assessment (PRA). See WASH-1400 for an example of this approach. The incidence rate can also be reduced due to the provision of better occupational health and safety programmes.[41]
Security risk
Security is freedom from, or resilience against, potential harm caused by others.
A security risk is "any event that could result in the compromise of organizational assets i.e. the unauthorized use, loss, damage, disclosure or modification of organizational assets for the profit, personal interest or political interests of individuals, groups or other entities."[42]
Security risk management involves protection of assets from harm caused by deliberate acts.
Risk is ubiquitous in all areas of life and we all manage these risks, consciously or intuitively, whether we are managing a large organization or simply crossing the road. Intuitive risk management is addressed under the psychology of risk below.
Risk management refers to a systematic approach to managing risks, and sometimes to the profession that does this. A general definition is that risk management consists of "coordinated activities to direct and control an organization with regard to risk".[3]
ISO 31000, the international standard for risk management,[4] describes a risk management process that consists of the following elements:
Risk treatment - selecting and implementing options for addressing risk.
Monitoring and reviewing
Recording and reporting
In general, the aim of risk management is to assist organizations in "setting strategy, achieving objectives and making informed decisions".[4] The outcomes should be "scientifically sound, cost-effective, integrated actions that [treat] risks while taking into account social, cultural, ethical, political, and legal considerations".[43]
In contexts where risks are always harmful, risk management aims to "reduce or prevent risks".[43] In the safety field it aims "to protect employees, the general public, the environment, and company assets, while avoiding business interruptions".[44]
For organizations whose definition of risk includes "upside" as well as "downside" risks, risk management is "as much about identifying opportunities as avoiding or mitigating losses".[45] It then involves "getting the right balance between innovation and change on the one hand, and avoidance of shocks and crises on the other".[46]
Risk assessment is a systematic approach to recognising and characterising risks, and evaluating their significance, in order to support decisions about how to manage them. ISO 31000 defines it in terms of its components as "the overall process of risk identification, risk analysis and risk evaluation".[4]
Risk assessment can be qualitative, semi-quantitative or quantitative:[4]
Qualitative approaches are based on qualitative descriptions of risks and rely on judgement to evaluate their significance.
Semi-quantitative approaches use numerical rating scales to group the consequences and probabilities of events into bands such as "high", "medium" and "low". They may use a risk matrix to evaluate the significance of particular combinations of probability and consequence.
Quantitative approaches, including Quantitative risk assessment (QRA) and probabilistic risk assessment (PRA), estimate probabilities and consequences in appropriate units, combine them into risk metrics, and evaluate them using numerical risk criteria.
The specific steps vary widely in different practice areas.
Risk identification
Risk identification is "the process of finding, recognizing and recording risks". It "involves the identification of risk sources, events, their causes and their potential consequences."[3]
ISO 31000 describes it as the first step in a risk assessment process, preceding risk analysis and risk evaluation.[4] In safety contexts, where risk sources are known as hazards, this step is known as "hazard identification".[47]
There are many different methods for identifying risks, including:[48]
Checklists or taxonomies based on past data or theoretical models.
Evidence-based methods, such as literature reviews and analysis of historical data.
Team-based methods that systematically consider possible deviations from normal operations, e.g. HAZOP, FMEA and SWIFT.
Empirical methods, such as testing and modelling to identify what might happen under particular circumstances.
Techniques encouraging imaginative thinking about possibilities of the future, such as scenario analysis.
Sometimes, risk identification methods are limited to finding and documenting risks that are to be analysed and evaluated elsewhere. However, many risk identification methods also consider whether control measures are sufficient and recommend improvements. Hence they function as stand-alone qualitative risk assessment techniques.
Risk analysis
Risk analysis is about developing an understanding of the risk. ISO defines it as "the process to comprehend the nature of risk and to determine the level of risk".[3] In the ISO 31000 risk assessment process, risk analysis follows risk identification and precedes risk evaluation. However, these distinctions are not always followed.
Determining the sources, causes and drivers of risk
Investigating the effectiveness of existing controls
Analysing possible consequences and their likelihood
Understanding interactions and dependencies between risks
Determining measures of risk
Verifying and validating results
Uncertainty and sensitivity analysis
Risk analysis often uses data on the probabilities and consequences of previous events. Where there have been few such events, or in the context of systems that are not yet operational and therefore have no previous experience, various analytical methods may be used to estimate the probabilities and consequences:
Proxy or analogue data from other contexts, presumed to be similar in some aspects of risk.
Risk evaluation involves comparing estimated levels of risk against risk criteria to determine the significance of the risk and make decisions about risk treatment actions.[48]
In most activities, risks can be reduced by adding further controls or other treatment options, but typically this increases cost or inconvenience. It is rarely possible to eliminate risks altogether without discontinuing the activity. Sometimes it is desirable to increase risks to secure valued benefits. Risk criteria are intended to guide decisions on these issues.[49]
Criteria that define the level of risk that can be accepted in pursuit of objectives, sometimes known as risk appetite, and evaluated by risk/reward analysis.[17]
Criteria that determine whether further controls are needed, such as benefit-cost ratio.
The simplest framework for risk criteria is a single level which divides acceptable risks from those that need treatment. This gives attractively simple results but does not reflect the uncertainties involved both in estimating risks and in defining the criteria.
Unacceptable risks – only permitted in exceptional circumstances.
Tolerable risks – to be kept as low as reasonably practicable (ALARP), taking into account the costs and benefits of further risk reduction.
Broadly acceptable risks – not normally requiring further reduction.
Descriptions of risk
There are many different risk metrics that can be used to describe or "measure" risk.
Triplets
Risk is often considered to be a set of triplets[21][17]
for i = 1,2,....,N
where:
is a scenario describing a possible event
is the probability of the scenario
is the consequence of the scenario
is the number of scenarios chosen to describe the risk
These are the answers to the three fundamental questions asked by a risk analysis:
What can happen?
How likely is it to happen?
If it does happen, what would the consequences be?
Risks expressed in this way can be shown in a table or risk register. They may be quantitative or qualitative, and can include positive as well as negative consequences.
The scenarios can be plotted in a consequence/likelihood matrix (or risk matrix). These typically divide consequences and likelihoods into 3 to 5 bands. Different scales can be used for different types of consequences (e.g. finance, safety, environment etc.), and can include positive as well as negative consequences.[48]
An updated version recommends the following general description of risk:[22]
where:
is an event that might occur
is the consequences of the event
is an assessment of uncertainties
is a knowledge-based probability of the event
is the background knowledge that U and P are based on
Probability distributions
If all the consequences are expressed in the same units (or can be converted into a consistent loss function), the risk can be expressed as a probability density function describing the "uncertainty about outcome":
One way of highlighting the tail of this distribution is by showing the probability of exceeding given losses, known as a complementary cumulative distribution function, plotted on logarithmic scales. Examples include frequency-number (FN) diagrams, showing the annual frequency of exceeding given numbers of fatalities.[48]
A simple way of summarizing the size of the distribution's tail is the loss with a certain probability of exceedance, such as the Value at Risk.
Expected values
Risk is often measured as the expected value of the loss. This combines the probabilities and consequences into a single value. See also expected utility. The simplest case is a binary possibility of Accident or No accident. The associated formula for calculating risk is then:
For example, if there is a probability of 0.01 of suffering an accident with a loss of $1000, then total risk is a loss of $10, the product of 0.01 and $1000.
In a situation with several possible accident scenarios, total risk is the sum of the risks for each scenario, provided that the outcomes are comparable:
In statistical decision theory, the risk function is defined as the expected value of a given loss function as a function of the decision rule used to make decisions in the face of uncertainty.
A disadvantage of defining risk as the product of impact and probability is that it presumes, unrealistically, that decision-makers are risk-neutral. A risk-neutral person's utility is proportional to the expected value of the payoff. For example, a risk-neutral person would consider 20% chance of winning $1 million exactly as desirable as getting a certain $200,000. However, most decision-makers are not actually risk-neutral and would not consider these equivalent choices.[17]Pascal's mugging is a philosophical thought experiment that demonstrates issues in assessing risk solely by the expected value of loss or return.
Volatility
In finance, volatility is the degree of variation of a trading price over time, usually measured by the standard deviation of logarithmic returns. Modern portfolio theory measures risk using the variance (or standard deviation) of asset prices. The risk is then:
The beta coefficient measures the volatility of an individual asset to overall market changes. This is the asset's contribution to systematic risk, which cannot be eliminated by portfolio diversification. It is the covariance between the asset's return ri and the market return rm, expressed as a fraction of the market variance:[51]
Outcome frequencies
Risks of discrete events such as accidents are often measured as outcome frequencies, or expected rates of specific loss events per unit time. When small, frequencies are numerically similar to probabilities, but have dimensions of [1/time] and can sum to more than 1. Typical outcomes expressed this way include:[52]
Individual risk - the frequency of a given level of harm to an individual.[53] It often refers to the expected annual probability of death, and is then comparable to the mortality rate.
Group (or societal risk) – the relationship between the frequency and the number of people suffering harm.[53]
Frequencies of property damage or total loss.
Frequencies of environmental damage such as oil spills.
Mortality risk
Many risks to people are expressed as probabilities of death. Since mortality risks are very small, they are sometimes converted to micromorts, defined as a one in a million chance of death, and hence 1 million times higher than the probability of death. In many cases, the risk depends on the time of exposure, and so is expressed as a mortality rate. Health risks, which vary widely with age, may be expressed as a loss of life expectancy.
Relative risk
In health, the relative risk is the ratio of the probability of an outcome in an exposed group to the probability of an outcome in an unexposed group.
An understanding that future events are uncertain and a particular concern about harmful ones may arise in anyone living in a community, experiencing seasons, hunting animals or growing crops. Most adults therefore have an intuitive understanding of risk. This may not be exclusive to humans.[54]
In ancient times, the dominant belief was in divinely determined fates, and attempts to influence the gods may be seen as early forms of risk management. Early uses of the word 'risk' coincided with an erosion of belief in divinely ordained fate.[55]
Risk perception is the subjective judgement that people make about the characteristics and severity of a risk. At its most basic, the perception of risk is an intuitive form of risk analysis.[56]
Heuristics and biases
Intuitive understanding of risk differs in systematic ways from accident statistics. When making judgements about uncertain events, people rely on a few heuristic principles, which convert the task of estimating probabilities to simpler judgements. These heuristics are useful but suffer from systematic biases.[57]
The "availability heuristic" is the process of judging the probability of an event by the ease with which instances come to mind. In general, rare but dramatic causes of death are over-estimated while common unspectacular causes are under-estimated.[58]
An "availability cascade" is a self-reinforcing cycle in which public concern about relatively minor events is amplified by media coverage until the issue becomes politically important.[59]
Despite the difficulty of thinking statistically, people are typically over-confident in their judgements. They over-estimate their understanding of the world and under-estimate the role of chance.[60] Even experts are over-confident in their judgements.[61]
Psychometric paradigm
The "psychometric paradigm" assumes that risk is subjectively defined by individuals, influenced by factors that can be elicited by surveys.[62] People's perception of the risk from different hazards depends on three groups of factors:
Dread – the degree to which the hazard is feared or might be fatal, catastrophic, uncontrollable, inequitable, involuntary, increasing or difficult to reduce.
Unknown - the degree to which the hazard is unknown to those exposed, unobservable, delayed, novel or unknown to science.
Number of people exposed.
Hazards with high perceived risk are in general seen as less acceptable and more in need of reduction.[63]
Cultural Theory views risk perception as a collective phenomenon by which different cultures select some risks for attention and ignore others, with the aim of maintaining their particular way of life.[64] Hence risk perception varies according to the preoccupations of the culture. The theory distinguishes variations known as "group" (the degree of binding to social groups) and "grid" (the degree of social regulation), leading to four world-views:[65]
Hierarchists (high group /high grid), who tend to approve of technology providing its risks are evaluated as acceptable by experts.
Egalitarians (high group/low grid), who tend to object to technology because it perpetuates inequalities that harm society and the environment.
Individualists (low group/low grid), who tend to approve of technology and see risks as opportunities.
Fatalists (low group/high grid), who do not knowingly take risks but tend to accept risks that are imposed on them
Cultural Theory helps explain why it can be difficult for people with different world-views to agree about whether a hazard is acceptable, and why risk assessments may be more persuasive for some people (e.g. hierarchists) than others. However, there is little quantitative evidence that shows cultural biases are strongly predictive of risk perception.[66]
Risk and emotion
The importance of emotion in risk
While risk assessment is often described as a logical, cognitive process, emotion also has a significant role in determining how people react to risks and make decisions about them.[67] Some argue that intuitive emotional reactions are the predominant method by which humans evaluate risk. A purely statistical approach to disasters lacks emotion and thus fails to convey the true meaning of disasters and fails to motivate proper action to prevent them.[68] This is consistent with psychometric research showing the importance of "dread" (an emotion) alongside more logical factors such as the number of people exposed.
The field of behavioural economics studies human risk-aversion, asymmetric regret, and other ways that human financial behaviour varies from what analysts call "rational". Recognizing and respecting the irrational influences on human decision making may improve naive risk assessments that presume rationality but in fact merely fuse many shared biases.
The "affect heuristic" proposes that judgements and decision-making about risks are guided, either consciously or unconsciously, by the positive and negative feelings associated with them.[69] This can explain why judgements about risks are often inversely correlated with judgements about benefits. Logically, risk and benefit are distinct entities, but it seems that both are linked to an individual's feeling about a hazard.[70]
Fear, anxiety and risk
Worry or anxiety is an emotional state that is stimulated by anticipation of a future negative outcome, or by uncertainty about future outcomes. It is therefore an obvious accompaniment to risk, and is initiated by many hazards and linked to increases in perceived risk. It may be a natural incentive for risk reduction. However, worry sometimes triggers behaviour that is irrelevant or even increases objective measurements of risk.[71]
Fear is a more intense emotional response to danger, which increases the perceived risk. Unlike anxiety, it appears to dampen efforts at risk minimisation, possibly because it provokes a feeling of helplessness.[72]
Dread risk
It is common for people to dread some risks but not others: They tend to be very afraid of epidemic diseases, nuclear power plant failures, and plane accidents but are relatively unconcerned about some highly frequent and deadly events, such as traffic crashes, household accidents, and medical errors. One key distinction of dreadful risks seems to be their potential for catastrophic consequences,[73] threatening to kill a large number of people within a short period of time.[74] For example, immediately after the 11 September attacks, many Americans were afraid to fly and took their car instead, a decision that led to a significant increase in the number of fatal crashes in the time period following the 9/11 event compared with the same time period before the attacks.[75][76]
Different hypotheses have been proposed to explain why people fear dread risks. First, the psychometric paradigm suggests that high lack of control, high catastrophic potential, and severe consequences account for the increased risk perception and anxiety associated with dread risks. Second, because people estimate the frequency of a risk by recalling instances of its occurrence from their social circle or the media, they may overvalue relatively rare but dramatic risks because of their overpresence and undervalue frequent, less dramatic risks.[76] Third, according to the preparedness hypothesis, people are prone to fear events that have been particularly threatening to survival in human evolutionary history.[77] Given that in most of human evolutionary history people lived in relatively small groups, rarely exceeding 100 people,[78] a dread risk, which kills many people at once, could potentially wipe out one's whole group. Indeed, research found[79] that people's fear peaks for risks killing around 100 people but does not increase if larger groups are killed. Fourth, fearing dread risks can be an ecologically rational strategy.[80] Besides killing a large number of people at a single point in time, dread risks reduce the number of children and young adults who would have potentially produced offspring. Accordingly, people are more concerned about risks killing younger, and hence more fertile, groups.[81]
Outrage is a strong moral emotion, involving anger over an adverse event coupled with an attribution of blame towards someone perceived to have failed to do what they should have done to prevent it. Outrage is the consequence of an event, involving a strong belief that risk management has been inadequate. Looking forward, it may greatly increase the perceived risk from a hazard.[82]
One of the growing areas of focus in risk management is the field of decision theory where behavioural and organizational psychology underpin our understanding of risk based decision making. This field considers questions such as "how do we make risk based decisions?", "why are we irrationally more scared of sharks and terrorists than we are of motor vehicles and medications?"
In decision theory, regret (and anticipation of regret) can play a significant part in decision-making, distinct from risk aversion[83][84] (preferring the status quo in case one becomes worse off).
Framing[85] is a fundamental problem with all forms of risk assessment. In particular, because of bounded rationality (our brains get overloaded, so we take mental shortcuts), the risk of extreme events is discounted because the probability is too low to evaluate intuitively. As an example, one of the leading causes of death is road accidents caused by drunk driving – partly because any given driver frames the problem by largely or totally ignoring the risk of a serious or fatal accident.
For instance, an extremely disturbing event (an attack by hijacking, or moral hazards) may be ignored in analysis despite the fact it has occurred and has a nonzero probability. Or, an event that everyone agrees is inevitable may be ruled out of analysis due to greed or an unwillingness to admit that it is believed to be inevitable. These human tendencies for error and wishful thinking often affect even the most rigorous applications of the scientific method and are a major concern of the philosophy of science.
Framing involves other information that affects the outcome of a risky decision. The right prefrontal cortex has been shown to take a more global perspective[86] while greater left prefrontal activity relates to local or focal processing.[87]
From the Theory of Leaky Modules[88] McElroy and Seta proposed that they could predictably alter the framing effect by the selective manipulation of regional prefrontal activity with finger tapping or monaural listening.[89] The result was as expected. Rightward tapping or listening had the effect of narrowing attention such that the frame was ignored. This is a practical way of manipulating regional cortical activation to affect risky decisions, especially because directed tapping or listening is easily done.
Psychology of risk taking
A growing area of research has been to examine various psychological aspects of risk taking. Researchers typically run randomised experiments with a treatment and control group to ascertain the effect of different psychological factors that may be associated with risk taking.[90] Thus, positive and negative feedback about past risk taking can affect future risk taking. In one experiment, people who were led to believe they are very competent at decision making saw more opportunities in a risky choice and took more risks, while those led to believe they were not very competent saw more threats and took fewer risks.[91]
People show risk aversion, so that they reject fair risky offers like a coin toss with an equal chance of winning and losing the same amount.[92] The expected premium for taking risks increases as the gambled amount increases.[93] Critically, people's intuitive response is often less risk-averse than their subsequent reflective response.[94]
Sex differences in financial decision making are relevant and significant. Numerous studies have found that women tend to be financially more risk-averse than men and hold safer portfolios.[95][96] A May 3, 2015 article in the Wall Street Journal by Georgette Jasen reported that "when it comes to investing, men sometimes have their way of doing things, and women have different ways."[97] Scholarly research has documented systematic differences in financial decisions such as buying investments versus insurance, donating to ingroups versus outgroups (such as terrorism victims in Iraq versus the United States), spending in stores,[98] and the endowment effect-or asking price for goods people have.[99]
Other considerations
Risk and uncertainty
In his seminal 1921 work Risk, Uncertainty, and Profit, Frank Knight established the distinction between risk and uncertainty.
... Uncertainty must be taken in a sense radically distinct from the familiar notion of Risk, from which it has never been properly separated. The term "risk," as loosely used in everyday speech and in economic discussion, really covers two things which, functionally at least, in their causal relations to the phenomena of economic organization, are categorically different. ... The essential fact is that "risk" means in some cases a quantity susceptible of measurement, while at other times it is something distinctly not of this character; and there are far-reaching and crucial differences in the bearings of the phenomenon depending on which of the two is really present and operating. ... It will appear that a measurable uncertainty, or "risk" proper, as we shall use the term, is so far different from an unmeasurable one that it is not in effect an uncertainty at all. We ... accordingly restrict the term "uncertainty" to cases of the non-quantitive type.[100]
Thus, Knightian uncertainty is immeasurable, not possible to calculate, while in the Knightian sense risk is measurable.
Another distinction between risk and uncertainty is proposed by Douglas Hubbard:[101][17]
Uncertainty: The lack of complete certainty, that is, the existence of more than one possibility. The "true" outcome/state/result/value is not known.
Measurement of uncertainty: A set of probabilities assigned to a set of possibilities. Example: "There is a 60% chance this market will double in five years."
Risk: A state of uncertainty where some of the possibilities involve a loss, catastrophe, or other undesirable outcome.
Measurement of risk: A set of possibilities each with quantified probabilities and quantified losses. Example: "There is a 40% chance the proposed oil well will be dry with a loss of $12 million in exploratory drilling costs."
In this sense, one may have uncertainty without risk but not risk without uncertainty. We can be uncertain about the winner of a contest, but unless we have some personal stake in it, we have no risk. If we bet money on the outcome of the contest, then we have a risk. In both cases there are more than one outcome. The measure of uncertainty refers only to the probabilities assigned to outcomes, while the measure of risk requires both probabilities for outcomes and losses quantified for outcomes.
Mild Versus Wild Risk
Benoit Mandelbrot distinguished between "mild" and "wild" risk and argued that risk assessment and analysis must be fundamentally different for the two types of risk.[102] Mild risk follows normal or near-normal probability distributions, is subject to regression to the mean and the law of large numbers, and is therefore relatively predictable. Wild risk follows fat-tailed distributions, e.g., Pareto or power-law distributions, is subject to regression to the tail (infinite mean or variance, rendering the law of large numbers invalid or ineffective), and is therefore difficult or impossible to predict. A common error in risk assessment and analysis is to underestimate the wildness of risk, assuming risk to be mild when in fact it is wild, which must be avoided if risk assessment and analysis are to be valid and reliable, according to Mandelbrot.
The terms risk attitude, appetite, and tolerance are often used similarly to describe an organisation's or individual's attitude towards risk-taking. One's attitude may be described as risk-averse, risk-neutral, or risk-seeking. Risk tolerance looks at acceptable/unacceptable deviations from what is expected.[clarification needed] Risk appetite looks at how much risk one is willing to accept. There can still be deviations that are within a risk appetite. For example, recent research finds that insured individuals are significantly likely to divest from risky asset holdings in response to a decline in health, controlling for variables such as income, age, and out-of-pocket medical expenses.[103]
Gambling is a risk-increasing investment, wherein money on hand is risked for a possible large return, but with the possibility of losing it all. Purchasing a lottery ticket is a very risky investment with a high chance of no return and a small chance of a very high return. In contrast, putting money in a bank at a defined rate of interest is a risk-averse action that gives a guaranteed return of a small gain and precludes other investments with possibly higher gain. The possibility of getting no return on an investment is also known as the rate of ruin.
Risk compensation is a theory which suggests that people typically adjust their behavior in response to the perceived level of risk, becoming more careful where they sense greater risk and less careful if they feel more protected.[104] By way of example, it has been observed that motorists drove faster when wearing seatbelts and closer to the vehicle in front when the vehicles were fitted with anti-lock brakes.
Risk and autonomy
The experience of many people who rely on human services for support is that 'risk' is often used as a reason to prevent them from gaining further independence or fully accessing the community, and that these services are often unnecessarily risk averse.[105] "People's autonomy used to be compromised by institution walls, now it's too often our risk management practices", according to John O'Brien.[106] Michael Fischer and Ewan Ferlie (2013) find that contradictions between formal risk controls and the role of subjective factors in human services (such as the role of emotions and ideology) can undermine service values, so producing tensions and even intractable and 'heated' conflict.[107]
Anthony Giddens and Ulrich Beck argued that whilst humans have always been subjected to a level of risk – such as natural disasters – these have usually been perceived as produced by non-human forces. Modern societies, however, are exposed to risks such as pollution, that are the result of the modernization process itself. Giddens defines these two types of risks as external risks and manufactured risks. The term Risk society was coined in the 1980s and its popularity during the 1990s was both as a consequence of its links to trends in thinking about wider modernity, and also to its links to popular discourse, in particular the growing environmental concerns during the period.
List of related books
This is a list of books about risk issues:
Title
Author(s)
Year
Acceptable Risk
Baruch Fischhoff, Sarah Lichtenstein, Paul Slovic, Steven L. Derby, and Ralph Keeney
state, even partial, of deficiency of information related to understanding or knowledge
Note 1: In some cases, uncertainty can be related to the organization’s context as well as to its objectives.
Note 2: Uncertainty is the root source of risk, namely any kind of “deficiency of information” that matters in relation to objectives (and objectives, in turn, relate to all relevant interested parties’ needs and expectations).
Note 1: An objective can be strategic, tactical or operational.
Note 2: Objectives can relate to different disciplines (such as financial, health and safety, and environmental goals) and can apply at different levels (such as strategic, organization-wide, project, product and process).
Note 3: An objective can be expressed in other ways, e.g. as an intended outcome, a purpose, an operational criterion, as a management system objective, or by the use of other words with similar meaning (e.g. aim, goal, target).
^Blount, Thomas (1661). Glossographia, or, A dictionary interpreting all such hard words of whatsoever language now used in our refined English tongue. London.
^ abcHansson, Sven Ove, "Risk", The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy (Fall 2018 Edition), Edward N. Zalta (ed.)
^A guide to the project management body of knowledge (PMBOK guide) (5th ed.). Project Management Institute. 2013. p. 309.
^Boroomand, A. and Smaldino, P.E., 2021. Hard Work, Risk-Taking, and Diversity in a Model of Collective Problem Solving. Journal of Artificial Societies and Social Simulation, 24(4).
^Julian Talbot and Miles Jakeman Security Risk Management Body of Knowledge, John Wiley & Sons, 2009.
^ abRisk Assessment and Risk Management in Regulatory Decision-Making. Presidential/Congressional Commission on Risk Assessment and Risk Management. 1997.
^"Risk management". Process Safety Glossary. Center for Chemical Process Safety. Retrieved 29 October 2020.
^AS/NZS 4360:1999 Risk Management. Standards Australia & Standards New Zealand. 1999.
^Risk: Improving government's capability to handle risk and uncertainty. Cabinet Office. 2002.
^Lyon, Bruce (2016). Fundamental Techniques. In Popov G, Lyon BK, Hollcraft B (eds.). Risk Assessment: A Practical Guide to Assessing Operational Risks: John Wiley & Sons.
^Kahneman, Daniel (2011). Thinking, Fast and Slow. London: Penguin Books. pp. 10–14.
^Slovic, Paul; Fischhoff, Baruch; Lichtenstein, Sarah (1979). "Rating the Risks". Environment. 2 (3): 14–20.
^Slovic, Paul (2000). The Perception of Risk. London: Earthscan. p. xxiii.
^Slovic, Paul (2000). The Perception of Risk. London: Earthscan. pp. 137–146.
^Douglas, Mary; Wildavsky, Aaron (1982). Risk and Culture: An Essay on the Selection of Technological and Environmental Dangers. Berkeley: University of California Press.
^ abLichtenstein, S; Slovic, P; Fischhoff, B; Layman, M; Combs, B (1978). "Judged frequency of lethal events". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory. 4 (6): 551–578. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.4.6.551. hdl:1794/22549.
^Öhman, A; Mineka, S (2001). "Fears, phobias, and preparedness: Toward an evolved module of fear and fear learning". Psychol Rev. 108 (3): 483–522. doi:10.1037/0033-295x.108.3.483. PMID11488376.
^Breakwell, Glynis (2014). The Psychology of Risk (2nd ed.). Cambridge University Press. p. 139.
^Virine, L., & Trumper, M. ProjectThink. Gower. 2013
^Virine, L., & Trumper, M. Project Risk Analysis Made Ridiculously Simple. World Scientific Publishing. 2017
^Amos Tversky / Daniel Kahneman, 1981. "The Framing of Decisions and the Psychology of Choice."[verification needed]
^Schatz, J.; Craft, S.; Koby, M.; DeBaun, M. R. (2004). "Asymmetries in visual-spatial processing following childhood stroke". Neuropsychology. 18 (2): 340–352. doi:10.1037/0894-4105.18.2.340. PMID15099156.
^Drake, R. A. (2004). Selective potentiation of proximal processes: Neurobiological mechanisms for spread of activation. Medical Science Monitor, 10, 231–234.
^Holt, C. A.; Laury, S. K. (2002). "Risk aversion and incentive effects". American Economic Review. 92 (5): 1644–1655. doi:10.1257/000282802762024700.
^Voudouri, A.; Białek, M.; De Neys, W. (2024). "Fast & slow decisions under risk: Intuition rather than deliberation drives advantageous choices". Cognition. 250: 105837. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105837.
^Bajtelsmit, Vickie L; Bernasek, Alexandra (1996). "Why Do Women Invest Differently Than Men?". Journal of Financial Counseling and Planning. 7: 1–10.
^Kurt, Didem; Inman, J. Jeffrey; Argo, Jennifer J. (2011). "The influence of friends on consumer spending: The role of agency-communion orientation and self-monitoring". Journal of Marketing Research. 48 (4): 741–754. doi:10.1509/jmkr.48.4.741. S2CID143542642.
^Dommer, Sara Loughran; Swaminathan, Vanitha (2013). "Explaining the endowment effect through ownership: The role of identity, gender, and self-threat". Journal of Consumer Research. 39 (5): 1034–1050. doi:10.1086/666737.
^Frank Hyneman Knight "Risk, uncertainty and profit" pg. 19, Hart, Schaffner, and Marx Prize Essays, no. 31. Boston and New York: Houghton Mifflin. 1921.
^Hubbard, Douglas (17 March 2014). How to Measure Anything: Finding the Value of Intangibles in Business. John Wiley & Sons. ISBN9781118539279.
^Mandelbrot, Benoit and Richard L. Hudson (2008). The (mis)Behaviour of Markets: A Fractal View of Risk, Ruin and Reward. London: Profile Books. ISBN978-1-84668-262-9.
Niklas Luhmann, 1996: Modern Society Shocked by its Risks (= University of Hong Kong, Department of Sociology Occasional Papers 17), Hong Kong, available via HKU Scholars HUB
Books
Historian David A. Moss' book When All Else Fails explains the US government's historical role as risk manager of last resort.
Bernstein P. L. Against the GodsISBN0-471-29563-9. Risk explained and its appreciation by man traced from earliest times through all the major figures of their ages in mathematical circles.
Rescher, Nicholas (1983). A Philosophical Introduction to the Theory of Risk Evaluation and Measurement. University Press of America.
Porteous, Bruce T.; Pradip Tapadar (December 2005). Economic Capital and Financial Risk Management for Financial Services Firms and Conglomerates. Palgrave Macmillan. ISBN978-1-4039-3608-0.
Gardner D. Risk: The Science and Politics of Fear, Random House Inc. (2008) ISBN0-7710-3299-4.
Novak S.Y. Extreme value methods with applications to finance. London: CRC. (2011) ISBN978-1-43983-574-6.
Hopkin P. Fundamentals of Risk Management. 2nd Edition. Kogan-Page (2012) ISBN978-0-7494-6539-1
Articles and papers
Cevolini, A (2015). ""Tempo e decisione. Perché Aristotele non-ha un concetto di rischio?" PDF". Divus Thomas. 118 (1): 221–249.
Clark, L.; Manes, F.; Antoun, N.; Sahakian, B. J.; Robbins, T. W. (2003). "The contributions of lesion laterality and lesion volume to decision-making impairment following frontal lobe damage". Neuropsychologia. 41 (11): 1474–1483. doi:10.1016/s0028-3932(03)00081-2. PMID12849765. S2CID46447795.
Drake, R. A. (1985). "Decision making and risk taking: Neurological manipulation with a proposed consistency mediation". Contemporary Social Psychology. 11: 149–152.
Drake, R. A. (1985). "Lateral asymmetry of risky recommendations". Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 11 (4): 409–417. doi:10.1177/0146167285114007. S2CID143899523.
Gregory, Kent J.; Bibbo, Giovanni; Pattison, John E. (2005). "A Standard Approach to Measurement Uncertainties for Scientists and Engineers in Medicine". Australasian Physical and Engineering Sciences in Medicine. 28 (2): 131–139. doi:10.1007/bf03178705. PMID16060321. S2CID13018991.
Hansson, Sven Ove. (2007). "Risk", The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy (Summer 2007 Edition), Edward N. Zalta (ed.), forthcoming [1].
Holton, Glyn A. (2004). "Defining Risk", Financial Analysts Journal, 60 (6), 19–25. A paper exploring the foundations of risk. (PDF file).
Knight, F. H. (1921) Risk, Uncertainty and Profit, Chicago: Houghton Mifflin Company. (Cited at: [2], § I.I.26.).
Kruger, Daniel J., Wang, X.T., & Wilke, Andreas (2007) "Towards the development of an evolutionarily valid domain-specific risk-taking scale" Evolutionary Psychology (PDF file).
Miller, L (1985). "Cognitive risk taking after frontal or temporal lobectomy I. The synthesis of fragmented visual information". Neuropsychologia. 23 (3): 359–369. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(85)90022-3. PMID4022303. S2CID45154180.
Miller, L.; Milner, B. (1985). "Cognitive risk taking after frontal or temporal lobectomy II. The synthesis of phonemic and semantic information". Neuropsychologia. 23 (3): 371–379. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(85)90023-5. PMID4022304. S2CID31082509.
Setiawan DjodiLahir13 Maret 1949 (umur 74)Surakarta, Jawa TengahPekerjaanpengusaha, penyanyiTahun aktif1989 - sekarangSuami/istriSandy Harun (cerai) Etty DjodyAnakMarimbi Djody Putri Natalie Esti Chandra Andini Djody Putri Maharani Djody Putri Shri Jenan Djody Putri Bey Mahayana Djody PutraOrang tuaK.R.T.H. Soedadi Tedjaningrat dan Kr. Ay. Soeprapti Soedadi K.P.H. Salahuddin Setiawan Djodi Nur Hadiningrat atau lebih dikenal sebagai Setiawan Djodi (lahir 13 Maret 1949) adalah seniman...
Argentine anthropological and cultural institute National Institute of Anthropology and Latin American ThoughtInstituto Nacional de Antropología y Pensamiento LatinoamericanoAgency overviewFormed1943HeadquartersBuenos Aires, ArgentinaParent departmentMinistry of Culture (Argentina)Websiteinapl.cultura.gob.ar The National Institute of Anthropology and Latin American Thought[1] (Spanish: Instituto Nacional de Antropología y Pensamiento Latinoamericano; INAPL) is an Argentine governmen...
Berikut ini adalah daftar jaringan dan stasiun televisi di Indonesia. Sejak berdirinya TVRI pada 1962, hingga 27 tahun setelah berdirinya TVRI, penduduk Indonesia hanya bisa menyaksikan satu saluran televisi saja. Namun pada tahun 1989, Pemerintah akhirnya mengizinkan RCTI sebagai stasiun televisi swasta pertama di Indonesia, meski hanya penduduk yang mempunyai dekoder yang dapat menyaksikan RCTI, walaupun pada akhirnya dibuka untuk masyarakat mulai tanggal 24 Agustus 1990. Televisi swasta (s...
Naturschutzgebiet Dhünntal und Linnefetal mit Seitentälern IUCN-Kategorie IV – Habitat/Species Management Area Nebenbach der Dhünn oberhalb von Loosenau Nebenbach der Dhünn oberhalb von Loosenau Lage Odenthal und Wermelskirchen, Nordrhein-Westfalen, Deutschland Fläche 213,31 Kennung GL-045 WDPA-ID 329323 Geographische Lage 51° 4′ N, 7° 10′ O51.0738547.164897160Koordinaten: 51° 4′ 26″ N, 7° 9′ 54″ O Naturschutzgebiet...
National Historic Landmark in Indianapolis, Indiana, U.S. Not to be confused with Das Deutsches Haus (San Francisco, California). United States historic placeAthenæum (Das Deutsche Haus)U.S. National Register of Historic PlacesU.S. National Historic Landmark The Athenæum in 2011.Show map of IndianapolisShow map of IndianapolisShow map of IndianaShow map of the United StatesLocation401 E. Michigan St., Indianapolis, IndianaCoordinates39°46′24″N 86°9′1″W / 39.77333°...
De Esso Manhattan in 1943. Het was een T2-tanker gebouwd door Sun Shipbuilding. USS Benevolence, een C4-type hospitaalschip gebouwd door Sun. Sun Shipbuilding & Drydock Co. (1917-1989) was een belangrijke scheepsbouwbedrijf in Chester, Pennsylvania. De werf lag ruim 20 kilometer ten zuiden van Philadelphia aan de Delaware. De werf was gespecialiseerd in de bouw van tankers. Tijdens de Tweede Wereldoorlog heeft zij veel T2-tankers gebouwd voor de United States Maritime Commission. Het bedr...
مايكل ديجين (بالألمانية: Michael Degen) معلومات شخصية الميلاد 31 يناير 1928[1] كيمنتس الوفاة 9 أبريل 2022 (94 سنة) [2][3] هامبورغ[4] مواطنة ألمانيا إسرائيل الأولاد إليزابيث ديجين [لغات أخرى] الحياة العملية المهنة ممثل مسرحي، وممثل أفلام،...
Tidak EnakAlbum studio karya NetralDirilis16 Februari 1997Direkam1996–1997GenreRock alternatifLabelBulletin MusikKronologi Netral Wa..lah (1995)Wa..lah1995 Tidak Enak (1997) Album Minggu Ini (1998)Album Minggu Ini1998 Tidak Enak merupakan sebuah album musik kedua karya Netral. Dirilis pada tahun 1997 dengan lagu utamanya yang berjudul Bobo.[1] Rilis album ini ditandai dengan konferensi pers di Jazz Rock Cafe Jakarta dan dihadiri hampir seluruh rekan pers di Jakarta dan rekan per...
Ermita de San Cristóbal Bien de Relevancia Local y Bien inmueble de Etnología LocalizaciónPaís España EspañaComunidad Comunidad Valenciana Comunidad ValencianaProvincia CastellónUbicación CastellnovoCoordenadas 39°52′05″N 0°27′33″O / 39.868049, -0.459194[editar datos en Wikidata] La Ermita de San Cristóbal sita en el término municipal de Castellnovo (Provincia de Castellón, España) se sitúa hacia el norte de la población y ...
Indian TV series or programme BreathePromotional posterGenreCrimeDramaThrillerStory byMayank SharmaAbhijeet DeshpandeDirected byMayank SharmaStarringR. MadhavanAmit SadhAtharva VishwakarmaMusic byAlokananda DasguptaCountry of originIndiaOriginal languageHindiNo. of seasons1No. of episodes8 (list of episodes)ProductionExecutive producerVikram MalhotraCamera setupS. BharathwajRunning time36-60 minutesProduction companyAbundantia EntertainmentOriginal releaseNetworkAmazon VideoRelease26...
This article uses bare URLs, which are uninformative and vulnerable to link rot. Please consider converting them to full citations to ensure the article remains verifiable and maintains a consistent citation style. Several templates and tools are available to assist in formatting, such as reFill (documentation) and Citation bot (documentation). (August 2022) (Learn how and when to remove this template message) 41°04′36″N 73°51′02″W / 41.076728°N 73.850449°W...
هذه المقالة تحتاج للمزيد من الوصلات للمقالات الأخرى للمساعدة في ترابط مقالات الموسوعة. فضلًا ساعد في تحسين هذه المقالة بإضافة وصلات إلى المقالات المتعلقة بها الموجودة في النص الحالي. (ديسمبر 2018) إن التصنيف الصناعي القياسي الدولي لجميع النشاطات الاقتصادية هو نظام تابع للأ...
В Википедии есть статьи о других людях с фамилией Сыроежкин. Григорий Сергеевич Сыроежкин Дата рождения 25 января 1900(1900-01-25) Место рождения село Волково, Балашовский уезд, Саратовская губерния Дата смерти 26 февраля 1939(1939-02-26) (39 лет) Место смерти Москва, СССР Принадлежн...
Hillfort in Kent, England The northeast slope of Bigbury Camp Bigbury Camp viewed from within the northern enclosure 3D view of the digital terrain model Bigbury Camp (formerly Bigberry Camp) is a univallate hill fort in the parish of Harbledown and Rough Common in Kent in England.[1] The fort is a Scheduled Ancient Monument, with a list entry identification number of 1005169.[2] Bigbury Camp is the only confirmed Iron Age hill fort in east Kent.[3] It is managed by Ke...
Head of the Maronite Church from 1796 to 1809 Joseph VII Tyan (يوسف السابع تيّان)Patriarch of AntiochChurchMaronite ChurchSeePatriarch of AntiochElectedApril 28, 1796Term ended1809PredecessorPhilip GemayelSuccessorJohn HelouOrdersOrdination(Priest) 1784Consecration(Bishop) August 6, 1786by Joseph EstephanPersonal detailsBornMarch 15, 1760BeirutDiedFebruary 20, 1820(1820-02-20) (aged 59)Qannubin Monastery, Kadisha Valley Joseph VII Peter Tyan[1] (born on March ...
Segunda Guerra do Congo Guerra Civil no Congo Civis esperando para atravessar a fronteira entre a RD Congo e o Ruanda (2001) Data 2 de agosto de 1998 – 18 de julho de 2003 Local República Democrática do Congo Desfecho Impasse militar Acordo de Sun City; Criação de um governo unificado e multipartidário no Congo, com Joseph Kabila como presidente e Jean-Pierre Bemba como primeiro-ministro; Acordo de Pretória; Ruanda retira suas tropas do Congo em troca de um compromisso internacional p...
Untuk kegunaan lain, lihat Alhambra dan Alhambra (disambiguasi). AlhambraSitus Warisan Dunia UNESCOLokasiGranada, Andalusia, SpanyolBagian dariAlhambra, Generalife dan Albayzín, GranadaKriteriaBudaya: i, iii, ivNomor identifikasi314-001Pengukuhan1984 (Sesi ke-8)Perluasan1994Koordinat37°10′37″N 3°35′24″W / 37.17695°N 3.59001°W / 37.17695; -3.59001Koordinat: 37°10′37″N 3°35′24″W / 37.17695°N 3.59001°W / 37.17695; -3.59001L...
You can help expand this article with text translated from the corresponding article in German. (September 2012) Click [show] for important translation instructions. View a machine-translated version of the German article. Machine translation, like DeepL or Google Translate, is a useful starting point for translations, but translators must revise errors as necessary and confirm that the translation is accurate, rather than simply copy-pasting machine-translated text into the English Wiki...
American lawyer This article needs additional citations for verification. Please help improve this article by adding citations to reliable sources. Unsourced material may be challenged and removed.Find sources: John William Wallace – news · newspapers · books · scholar · JSTOR (January 2021) (Learn how and when to remove this message)John William Wallaceportrait by John NeagleBorn1815 Died1884 (aged 68–69) John William Wallace (February 17...
Men's water poloat the Games of the III OlympiadWater polo game during 1904 Summer Olympics.VenueU.S. Life Saving Exhibit LakeDateSeptember 5 (semifinal) September 6 (final)Competitors21 from 1 nationMedalists New York Athletic Club United States Chicago Athletic Association United States Missouri Athletic Club United States← 19001908 → At the 1904 Summer Olympics, a water polo tournament was contested, with three club teams of seven players eac...