The neural basis of prey detection, recognition, and orientation was studied in depth by Jörg-Peter Ewert in a series of experiments that made the toad visual system a model system in neuroethology (neural basis of natural behavior). He began by observing the natural prey catching behavior of the common European toad (Bufo bufo).
Ewert's work with toads yielded several important discoveries (Ewert 1974, 2004). In general, his research revealed the specific neural circuits for recognition of complex visual stimuli. Specifically, he identified two main regions of the brain, the tectum and the thalamic-pretectal region, that were responsible for discriminating prey from non-prey and revealed the neural pathways that connected them. Furthermore, he found that the neural mechanisms are plastic and adaptable to varying environments and conditions (Carew 2000; Zupanc 2004).
The common toad responds to a moving insect or worm with a series of prey-catching reactions: (1) orienting towards prey, (2) stalking up to prey, (3) binocular fixation, (4) snapping, (5) swallowing and (6) mouth-wiping with forelimb (Ewert 1974). This series of movement constitutes a stimulus-response chain, where each reaction of the toad provides the stimulus constellation for the next response. First, if an object is recognized as prey and thus catches the toad's attention, the toad will orient towards the stimulus by turning its body to face it. Then it approaches the prey, binocularly focusing intently on it. During the attack, it snaps at the object with its tongue or jaws and swallows it, using the ability to perceive depth, stereopsis, to aid in the feat. Finally, it wipes its mouth with a forelimb. These actions constitute a series of well-defined behavioral patterns.
One reason for this type of stimulus-response chain is that, unlike humans, toads do not have involuntary saccadic eye movements and they also cannot perform "tracking eye movements" (Ewert 1980). They must, therefore, depend on recognizing the stimulus before they respond. As a result, they have developed a specific detection system that, for example, allows them to discriminate between edible prey and dangerous predators.
The lack of saccadic eye movements forces the toad to hold its eyes in rigid positions. Therefore, it must decide whether the object is "prey" or "non-prey" before moving itself. If it orients towards an object, it must already have decided "prey" and then commits itself to snapping by reducing the thresholds for subsequent prey-catching responses. Even when the prey stimulus quickly disappeared after orienting, the aroused toad may sometimes complete the subsequent responses.
Prey vs. predator response
When a toad is presented with a moving stimulus, it generally may react with one of two responses. Depending on the size and the configuration of the stimulus, it will either engage in orienting (prey-catching) behavior or avoidance (escape) behavior, which consists of "planting-down" defensive postures or a crouching avoidance response.
In determining the size of a stimulus, a toad will consider both the angular size, which is measured in degrees of visual angle, and the absolute size, which takes into consideration the distance between the toad and the object. This second ability, to judge absolute size by estimating distance, is known as size constancy.
To study behavioral responses of toads to varying types of stimuli, Ewert conducted experiments by placing the toad in the center of a small cylindrical glass vessel. He then rotated a small stripe (bar) of contrasting cardboard (acting as a visual 'dummy') around the vessel to mimic either prey-like or threat-like stimuli; see Video. The rate of turning was recorded as a measure of orienting behavior (prey-catching activity). By changing characteristics of the visual stimulus in a methodical manner, Ewert was able to comprehensively study the key features that determine behavior.
Up to a certain size, squares rotated around the toad successfully elicited prey-catching responses. Toads avoided large squares. Vertical bars nearly never elicited prey-catching behavior and they were increasingly ineffective with increasing height. Horizontal bars, in contrast, were very successful at eliciting prey-catching behavior and their effectiveness increased with increasing length, to a certain degree. Additional vertical segments on top of horizontal bars significantly decreased prey-catching responses. In general, movement of a rectangle in the direction of its long axis is perceived by the toad to be wormlike, whereas movement along the short axis is interpreted as anti-wormlike. By means of a different experimental setup it was shown that the worm vs. anti-worm discrimination is independent (invariant) of the direction the object moves in the toad's visual field.
Stationary objects usually elicit no prey-catching or avoidance responses. However, toads in an untextured surrounding cannot distinguish between the retinal image of a small moving object and the retinal image of a small stationary object induced by the toad's own motion. Both images elicit prey-capture. In a textured surrounding, however, the induced moving retinal image is ignored.
In addition, the contrast between stimuli and background can significantly affect the type of behavior. In response to a wormlike stripe, common toads orient and snap towards the edge leading in the direction of motion, given that the stripe is black and the background white. If the stimulus/background contrast is reversed, the toad prefers the trailing edge of the white stripe and often snaps behind it. Obviously, "off"-effects (rapid change in luminance from bright to dark) by the moving contrast borders play a guiding role. Generally, white square objects moving against a black background are more attractive as prey than black objects on white. However, this tendency is plastic and reverses seasonally, where black objects against a white background are much more effective at eliciting prey-catching behavior in the fall and winter (Ewert 1980).
Feature detectors and the visual system
To understand the neural mechanisms underlying the toad's behavioral responses, Ewert performed a series of recording and stimulation experiments. First and foremost, the results allowed him to understand the way the visual system is constructed and connected to the central nervous system. Secondly, he discovered areas of the brain that were responsible for differential analysis of stimuli.
First, the retina is connected to the optic tectum by at least three types of ganglion cells, each with an excitatory receptive field and a surrounding inhibitory receptive field, but they differ in the diameter of their central excitatory receptive fields. Diameters in Class II (R2) ganglion cells are approximately four degrees visual angle. Those in Class III (R3) cells are about eight degrees and Class IV (R4) ganglion cells range from twelve to fifteen degrees. As stimuli move across the toad's visual field, information is sent to the optic tectum in the toad's midbrain. The optic tectum exists as an ordered localization system, in the form of a topographical map. Each point on the map corresponds to a particular region of the toad's retina and thus its entire visual field. Likewise, when a spot on the tectum was electrically stimulated, the toad would turn toward a corresponding part of its visual field, providing further evidence of the direct spatial connections.
Among Ewert's many experimental goals was the identification of feature detectors, neurons that respond selectively to specific features of a sensory stimulus. Results showed that there were no "worm-detectors" or "enemy-detectors" at the level of the retina. Instead, he found that the optic tectum and the thalamic-pretectal region (in the diencephalon) play significant roles in the analysis and interpretation of visual stimuli (summarized in Ewert 1974, 2004; Ewert and Schwippert 2006).
Electrical stimulation experiments demonstrated that the tectum initiates orienting and snapping behaviors. It contains many different visually sensitive neurons, among these Type I and Type II neurons (later named T5.1 and T5.2, respectively). Type I neurons are activated when an object traversing the toad's visual field is extended in the direction of movement; Type II neurons, too, but they will fire less when the object is extended in a direction that is perpendicular to the direction of movement. Those T5.2 neurons display prey-selective properties; see prey feature detectors. The discharge patterns of these neurons – recorded in freely moving toads – "predict" prey-catching reactions, e.g. the tongue flip of snapping. Their axons project down to the bulbar/spinal motor systems, such as the hypoglossal nucleus which harbors the motor neurons of the tongue muscles. In combination with additional projection neurons, prey-selective cells contribute to the ability of the tectum to initiate orienting behavior and snapping, respectively.
The thalamic-pretectal region initiates avoidance behavior in the toad. More specifically, electrical triggering the thalamic-pretectal region initiates a variety of protective movements such as eyelid closing, ducking and turning away (Ewert 1974, 2004). Various types of neurons in this region are responsible for the avoidance behaviors and they are all sensitive to different types of stimuli. One type of neurons (TH3) is activated by large threatening objects, especially those ones that are extended perpendicularly to the direction of motion. Another type (TH6) is activated by a looming object moving toward the toad. Still other types (TH10) respond to large stationary obstacles, and there are also neurons responding to stimulation of the balance sensors in the toad's ear. Stimulation of (a combination of) such types of neurons would cause the toad to display different kinds of protective behaviors.
Lesioning experiments led to the discovery of pathways extending between the tectum and the thalamic-pretectal region. When the tectum was removed, orienting behavior disappeared. When the thalamic-pretectal region was removed, avoidance behavior was entirely absent while orienting behavior was enhanced even to predator stimuli. Furthermore, prey-selective properties were impaired both in prey-selective neurons and in prey-catching behavior (Zupanc 2004). Finally, when one half of the thalamic-pretectal region was removed, the disinhibition applied to the entire visual field of the opposite eye. These and other experiments suggest that pathways, involving axons of type TH3 cells, extend from the pretectal thalamus to the tectum, suitable to modulate tectal responses to visual stimuli and to determine prey-selective properties due to inhibitory influences.
Modulatory loops and evolutionary perspectives
Having analyzed neuronal processing streams in brain structures (pretectal, tectal, medullary) that mediate between visual stimuli and adequate behavioral responses in toads, Ewert and coworkers examined various neural loops that—in connection with certain forebrain structures (striatal, pallial, thalamic)—can initiate, modulate or modify stimulus-response mediation (Ewert and Schwippert 2006). For example, in the course of associative learning the toad's visual prey schema can be modified to include non-prey objects. After lesions to a telencephalic structure involved in learning—the posterior ventromedial pallium—this learning effect fails and prey recognition shows again its species-specific selectivity. The posterior part of the ventromedial pallium is homologous to the hippocampus of mammals which is also involved in learning processes. Both in anuran amphibians and mammals striatal efferents are, for example, involved in directed attention, i.e. gating an orienting response towards a sensory stimulus. The anuran striatum is homologous to a portion of the amniote basal ganglia.
From an evolutionary point of view it is important to note that the tetrapod vertebrates share a common pattern of homologous brainstem and forebrain structures (e.g., see Reiner et al. 1998; González et al. 1999; Kenigfest et al. 2002). Neuroethological, neuroanatomical, and neurochemical investigations suggest that the neural networks underlying essential functions—such as attention, orienting, approaching, avoidance, associative or non-associative learning, and basic motor skills—have, so to speak, a phylogenetic origin in homologous structures of the amphibian brain.
From a neural network approach, it is reasonable to ask whether the toad's ability to classify moving objects by special configuration cues—object's dimension parallel vs. perpendicular to the direction of motion—is unique in the animal kingdom. Developmental studies suggest that this detection principle is an adaptation in terrestrialamphibians to their biotope and thus addressed to objects that are moving on land. Actually, it was found that this specific detection ability fails to occur in aquatic frogs (Rana esculenta). In common toads (Bufo bufo) it was shown that this detection property matures during the metamorphosis of the aquatic tadpoles in the course of their transition to terrestrial life (independent of food experience). Comparable detection principles are discovered in amphibious fish (Periophthalmus koelreuteri) and in insects(Sphodromantis lineola). In mammals, erect body postures, for example, may address a threat signal to a rival. This suggests that the configuration-algorithm responsible for the distinction between profitable (e.g., prey-like) vs. dangerous (e.g., threat-like) may be implemented by quite different neural networks. Studies in artificial neuronal nets support this presumption (for an example see Ewert 2004).
Stereopsis in toads
Previously thought to be characteristic of solely primates and mammals with front-facing eyes, the ability to process depth information from multiple visual points in space has been determined to be possessed by most amphibians, namely, frogs and toads (Nityananda and Read 2017). From an evolutionary point of view, there has been substantial support for the idea that stereo vision has evolved as a natural progression for animals with binocular vision, meaning, a substantial distance between the two eyes (Nityananda and Read 2017). This theory would mean that stereopsis has evolved independently at least four times to account for stereo vision being present in mammals, birds, amphibians, and some invertebrates.
For toads in particular, stereopsis would prove massively advantageous, as depth perception would be particularly useful in aiding in catching prey and calculating distance.
Ewert, J.-P. (1980). Neuroethology: an introduction to the neurophysiological fundamentals of behavior. Springer-Verlag, Berlin/Heidelberg/New York.
Ewert J.-P. (2004) Motion perception shapes the visual world of amphibians. In: Prete F.R. (Ed.) Complex Worlds from Simpler Nervous Systems. Cambridge, MA, MIT Press, pp. 117–160
Ewert J.-P., Schwippert W.W. (2006) Modulation of visual perception and action by forebrain structures and their interactions in amphibians. In: Levin E.D. (ed.) Neurotransmitter Interactions and Cognitive Function. Birkhäuser, Basel, pp. 99–136
González A., Smeets W.J., Marín O. (1999). Evidences for shared features in the organization of the basal ganglia in tetrapods: studies in amphibians. Eur. J. Morphol., 37(2-3), 151-154.
Kenigfest N.B., Belekhova M.G., Karamyan O.A., Minakova M.N., Rio J.-P., Reperant J. (2002). Neurochemical organization of the turtle protectum: an immunohistochemical study. Comparative analysis. J. Evol. Biochem. Physiol., 38(6), 673-688.
Nityananda, V., and J. C. A. Read. 2017. Stereopsis in animals: evolution, function and mechanisms. Journal of Experimental Biology 220:2502–2512.
Torre CabreraUbicazioneStatoRegno di Sicilia Stato attuale Italia RegioneSicilia CittàMarina di Ragusa IndirizzoPiazza delle Torre di Mazzarelli Coordinate36°46′58.94″N 14°33′16.07″E / 36.783038°N 14.554465°E36.783038; 14.554465Coordinate: 36°46′58.94″N 14°33′16.07″E / 36.783038°N 14.554465°E36.783038; 14.554465 Informazioni generaliTipoTorre costiera Inizio costruzioneXVI secolo voci di architetture militari presenti su Wikipedia ...
الملك الكوشي في نبتة انلماني تمثال الملك انلماني (المتحف البريطاني, لندن) الملك الكوشي في نبتة مرافق ميديكن ؟ معلومات شخصية الاسم الكامل انلماني تاريخ الميلاد القرن 7 ق.م تاريخ الوفاة سنة 593 ق م مكان الدفن مدينة نوري ( الهرم رقم 8) الأب سنكامانسكن الأم الملكة نسالسا تعد...
أسرة تيودورThe Tudors التعليق الملك هنري الثامن وخلفه زوجاته الستة النوع دراما تاريخية صناعة مايكل هيرست بطولة جوناثان ريس مايرزسام نيلهنري كافيلإلينور وليامناتالي دورمرماريا دويل كينيديكالوم بلوهنري تشيرنينك دانينغجيمس فراينجيمي توماس كينغهانز ماثيسونأنابيل واليسألان فا
Halaman ini berisi artikel tentang negara di Amerika Utara. Untuk bahasa yang dituturkan di India, lihat bahasa Kannada. KanadaCanada (Inggris dan Prancis) Bendera Lambang Semboyan: A Mari Usque Ad Mare (Latin: Dari Laut ke Laut)Lagu kebangsaan: O Canada (Indonesia: Oh Kanada) Lagu kerajaan: God Save the King (Indonesia: Tuhan Menjaga sang Raja) Perlihatkan BumiPerlihatkan peta BenderaIbu kotaOttawa45°24′N 75°40′W / 45.400°N 75.667°W / 45.400; ...
Wiedervernässte Fläche im Toten Moor mit abgestorbenen Birken Wiedervernässung mittels breiter Torfdämme im Hahnenmoor Unterschiede zwischen intakten und wieder vernässten Mooren Einfluss der Moorwiedervernässung auf verschmutzte Gewässer Unter Wiedervernässung werden im Naturschutz und in der Landschaftspflege Maßnahmen zur Anhebung des Wasserstandes in Feuchtgebieten wie Mooren, Feuchtwiesen oder Flussauen mit dem Ziel der Wiederherstellung oder Renaturierung dieser Ökosysteme ver...
Blick auf Allington Castle Allington Castle ist eine Burg im Dorf Allington, direkt nördlich von Maidstone in der englischen Grafschaft Kent. Die erste Burg an dieser Stelle war eine nicht autorisierte Festung, die während der Anarchie Anfang des 12. Jahrhunderts errichtet und später im selben Jahrhundert wieder abgerissen wurde, als die königliche Kontrolle wieder etabliert worden war. Sie wurde durch ein Herrenhaus ersetzt, das dann mit königlicher Erlaubnis im 13. Jahrhundert befestig...
禾 Hòa (115)(114)禸 (Nhựu)◄ 「禾」 ►穴 (Huyệt)(116)Bảng mã Unicode: 禾 (U+79BE) [1]Giải nghĩa: lúaBính âm:héChú âm phù hiệu:ㄏㄜˊQuốc ngữ La Mã tự:herWade–Giles:ho2Phiên âm Quảng Đông theo Yale:wòhViệt bính:wo4Bạch thoại tự:hôKana:カ kaいね, のぎ ine, nogiKanji:ノ木偏 nogihenHangul:벼 byeoHán-Hàn:화 hwaHán-Việt:hòaCách viết: gồm 5 nét Bộ Hòa, bộ thứ 115 có nghĩa là lúa l...
American animal rights organization PETA redirects here. For other uses, see PETA (disambiguation). The neutrality of this article is disputed. Relevant discussion may be found on the talk page. Please do not remove this message until conditions to do so are met. (October 2023) (Learn how and when to remove this template message) People for the Ethical Treatment of AnimalsLogo used since 1980FoundedMarch 22, 1980; 43 years ago (1980-03-22)FoundersIngrid NewkirkAlex PachecoTy...
Bósnia e Herzegovina Basquetebol Informações gerais Federação Federação de Basquetebol da Bósnia e Herzegovina Sigla FIBA BIH Confederação FIBA Europa Ranking FIBA 42°[1] Treinador Adis Bećiragić Jogos Olímpicos Participações 0 (Primeira em -) Melhor - Última - (-) Campeonato Mundial Participações 0 (Primeira em -) Melhor - Última - (-) EuroBasket Participações 9 (Primeira em 1993) Melhor 8º (1993) Última 23º (2015) 1º uniforme 2º uniforme A Seleção Bósnia de Bas...
130 AD RegimentActive1967 – presentCountry IndiaAllegianceIndiaBranch Indian ArmyTypeCorps of Army Air DefenceSizeRegimentMotto(s)Sanskrit: आकाशे शत्रुन् जहि English: Defeat the Enemy in the SkyColors Sky Blue and RedAnniversaries28 January (Raising Day)Equipment40 mm L/70 Air Defence GunInsigniaAbbreviation130 AD RegtMilitary unit 130 Air Defence Regiment is part of the Corps of Army Air Defence of the Indian Army. Formation 130 Air Defence Regime...
Суперкубок Австрії з футболу 1995Турнір Суперкубок Австрії з футболу Аустрія (Зальцбург) Рапід (Відень) 2 1 Дата 26 липня 1995Стадіон Альпенштадіон, КапфенбергАрбітр Манфред РіцінгерГлядачі 6 500← 1994 1996 → Суперкубок Австрії з футболу 1995 — 10-й розіграш турніру. Матч відбу...
American rapper and singer (born 1981) Travie McCoyMcCoy at Warped Tour in 2008Background informationBirth nameTravis Lazarus McCoyAlso known as Schleprok Bernie Allen Born (1981-08-05) August 5, 1981 (age 42)Geneva, New York, U.S.Genres Pop-rap rap rock pop rock alternative hip hop[1] Occupation(s) Rapper singer songwriter Years active1997–presentLabels Hopeless Decaydance Fueled by Ramen Nappy Boy Atlantic Member of Gym Class Heroes Websitetraviemccoy.comMusical artist Travis...
Guinean footballer Naby Diarso (born 1 January 1977 in Conakry) is a Guinean footballer, who currently plays with Satellite FC.[1] International career He represented Guinea at the 2006 Africa Cup of Nations and 2008 games.[2] References ^ Naby Diarso at National-Football-Teams.com ^ African Cup of Nations - CAN 2008 - Afrik-news.com vteGuinea squad – 2006 Africa Cup of Nations 1 Diarso 2 Feindouno 3 I. Camara 4 Kaba 5 Baldé 6 Thiam 7 Mans...
Artikel ini sudah memiliki daftar referensi, bacaan terkait, atau pranala luar, tetapi sumbernya belum jelas karena belum menyertakan kutipan pada kalimat. Mohon tingkatkan kualitas artikel ini dengan memasukkan rujukan yang lebih mendetail bila perlu. (Pelajari cara dan kapan saatnya untuk menghapus pesan templat ini)Drs. H.Syahfan Badri SampurnoAnggota Dewan Perwakilan Rakyat Republik IndonesiaMasa jabatan1 Oktober 2009 – 1 Oktober 2014Daerah pemilihanBengkulu Informasi pribadiLa...
شارنوز-سور-أين شعار الاسم الرسمي (بالفرنسية: Charnoz-sur-Ain)(بالفرنسية: Charnoz)(بالفرنسية: Charnoz)[1] الإحداثيات 45°51′51″N 5°13′21″E / 45.864166666667°N 5.2225°E / 45.864166666667; 5.2225[2] [3] تقسيم إداري البلد فرنسا[4] التقسيم الأعلى آن خصائص جغرافية &...
Who's in a Family? AuthorRobert SkutchIllustratorLaura NienhausPublisherTricycle PressPublication date1995Pages32ISBN1-883672-66-XOCLC30979383 Who's in a Family? is a children's book which depicts a variety of non-traditional families, including interracial, single-parent, and families with gay and lesbian partners as parents. It intentionally emphasizes the normalcy of different family arrangements.[1] Background Robert Skutch is an American author born in 1925 and has published mult...
2006 single by Jeannie Ortega featuring PapooseCrowdedSingle by Jeannie Ortega featuring Papoosefrom the album No Place Like BKLYN ReleasedFebruary 21, 2006Recorded2005GenreR&BLength3:17LabelHollywoodSongwriter(s) Mikkel Eriksen Tor Erik Hermansen Willie Mackie Jeannie Ortega Producer(s)StargateJeannie Ortega singles chronology It's R Time(2005) Crowded(2006) So Done(2006) Papoose singles chronology Touch It(2006) Crowded(2006) Ridin'(2006) Crowded is a song by Jeannie Ortega rele...
Battle of AsfeldAlboin, son of King AudoinAlboin slew Thorismund in single combat during the battle [1]Date552LocationAsfeld?, Pannonia SecundaResult Lombard victoryBelligerents Lombards GepidsCommanders and leaders King AudoinPrince Alboin[2][3] King ThorisindPrince Thorismund †[2][3]Casualties and losses Unknown Heavy[3] The Battle of Asfeld was fought in 552 between the Lombards and the Gepids. The Lombards, led by King Audoin (wit...
Teluk BerauTeluk MacCleurSemenanjung Vogelkop dan Bomberai, sebelah barat Teluk Bintuni adalah Teluk Berau, Mulut Naga Papua Barat.Aliran masuk utamaTeluk BintuniAliran keluar utamaLaut Seram Teluk Berau, sebelumnya Teluk MacCleur adalah sebuah teluk yang memisahkan Vogelkop (Doberai) dan Semenanjung Bomberai dari Nugini Barat. Memiliki awal di Laut Seram di sebelah barat dan berakhir di Teluk Bintuni di sebelah timur. Geografi Teluk Berau terbentuk dari pelebaran Teluk Bintuni ke arah timur ...
8th IIFA AwardsThe official logo of the 2007 IIFA AwardsDate7 June 2007 (2007-06-07)–9 June 2007 (2007-06-09)SiteHallam FM Arena Sheffield, EnglandHosted by Boman Irani Lara Dutta HighlightsBest PictureRang De BasantiBest DirectionRajkumar Hirani(Lage Raho Munna Bhai)Best ActorHrithik Roshan(Krrish)Best ActressRani Mukerji(Kabhi Alvida Naa Kehna)Most awardsRang De Basanti (11)Most nominationsRang De Basanti (21)Television coverageChannelStar PlusNetworkSTAR TV ...