Tend-and-befriend is a purported behavior exhibited by some animals, including humans, in response to threat. It refers to protection of offspring (tending) and seeking out their social group for mutual defense (befriending). In evolutionary psychology, tend-and-befriend is theorized as having evolved as the typical female response to stress.
According to the Polyvagal theory developed by Dr. Stephen Porges, the "Social Nervous System" is an affiliative neurocircuitry that prompts affiliation, particularly in response to stress.[2] This system is described as regulating social approach behavior. A biological basis for this regulation appears to be oxytocin.[3]
Oxytocin has been tied to a broad array of social relationships and activities, including peer bonding, sexual activity, and affiliative preferences.[3] Oxytocin is released in humans in response to a broad array of stressors, especially those that may trigger affiliative needs. Oxytocin promotes affiliative behavior, including maternal tending and social contact with peers.[4] Thus, affiliation under stress serves tending needs, including protective responses towards offspring. Affiliation may also take the form of befriending, namely seeking social contact for one's own protection, the protection of offspring, and the protection of the social group. These social responses to threat reduce biological stress responses, including lowering heart rate, blood pressure, and hypothalamic pituitary adrenal axis (HPA) stress activity, such as cortisol responses.[5]
According to some research, women are more likely to respond to stress through tending and befriending than men.[citation needed] Paralleling this behavioral sex difference, estrogen enhances the effects of oxytocin, whereas androgens inhibit oxytocin release.[6]
Tending under stress
Female stress responses that increased offspring survival would have led to higher fitness and thus were more likely to be passed on through natural selection.[1] In the presence of threats, protecting and calming offspring while blending into the environment may have increased chances of survival for mother and child. When faced with stress, females often respond by tending to offspring, which in turn reduces stress levels. Studies conducted by Repetti (1989) show that mothers respond to highly stressful workdays by providing more nurturing behaviors towards their children.[7] In contrast, fathers who experienced stressful workdays were more likely to withdraw from their families or were more interpersonally conflictual that evening at home. Furthermore, physical contact between mothers and their offspring following a threatening event decreased HPA activity and sympathetic nervous system arousal.[8] Oxytocin, released in response to stressors, may be the mechanism underlying the female caregiving response. Studies of ewes show that administration of oxytocin promoted maternal behavior.[9] Breastfeeding in humans, which is associated with maternal oxytocin release, is physiologically calming to both mothers and infants.[1]
Cooperative breeding
Tend-and-befriend is a critical, adaptive strategy that is hypothesized to have enhanced reproductive success among female cooperative breeders. Cooperative breeders are group-living animals where infant and juvenile care from non-mother helpers are essential to offspring survival.[10] Cooperative breeders include wolves, elephants, many nonhuman primates, and humans. Among all primates and most mammals, endocrinological and neural processes lead females to nurture infants, including unrelated infants, after being exposed long enough to infant signals.[11] Non-mother female wolves and wild dogs sometimes begin lactating to nurse the alpha female's pups.
Humans are born helpless and altricial, mature slowly, and depend on parental investment well into their young adult lives, and often even later.[11] Humans have spent most of human evolution as hunter-gathererforagers. Among foraging societies without modern birth control methods, women tend to give birth about every four years during their reproductive lifespan.[11] When mothers give birth, they often have multiple dependent children in their care, who rely on adults for food and shelter for years. Such a reproductive strategy would not have been able to evolve if women did not have help from others. Allomothers (helpers who are not a child's mother) often protect, provision, carry, and care for children.[11] Allomothers are usually a child's aunts, uncles, fathers, grandmothers, siblings, and other persons in the community. Even in modern Western societies, parents often rely on family members, friends, and babysitters to help care for children. Burkart, Hrdy, and Van Schaik (2009) argue that cooperative breeding in humans may have led to the evolution of psychological adaptations for greater prosociality, enhanced social cognition, and cognitive abilities for cooperative purposes, including willingness to share mental states and shared intentionality.[10] These cognitive, prosocial processes brought on by cooperative breeding may have led to the emergence of culture and language.
Befriending under stress
Group living provides numerous benefits, including protection from predators and cooperation to achieve shared goals and access to resources. In modernized societies at least, it is found that women create, maintain, and use social networks—especially friendships with other women—to manage stressful conditions.[1] During threatening situations, group members can be a source of support and protection for women and their children. Research shows that women operating in a modern and westernized paradigm are more likely to seek the company of others in times of stress, compared to men.[12] In some cultures, women and adolescent girls report more sources of social support and are more likely to turn to same-sex peers for support than men or boys are. One study of six cultures (five of whom were non-western) found that women and girls tend to provide more frequent and effective support than men do, and they are more likely to seek help and support from other female friends and family members, although there was a degree of cultural variation based on the metric used.[13] Women tend to affiliate with other women under stressful situations. However, when women were given a choice to either wait alone or to affiliate with an unfamiliar man before a stressful laboratory challenge, they chose to wait alone.[1] Female-female social networks can provide assistance for childcare, exchange of resources, and protection from predators, other threats, and other group members. Smuts (1992) and Taylor et al. (2000) argue that female social groups also provide protection from male aggression.[1][14] In spite of the large cultural diversity within this six-culture sample, none of the societies included demonstrated matrilineal tendencies, which have been found to negate and cancel out many supposedly "universal" sex differences (see "criticism" section below). Additionally, the metrics used by the Whitings for evaluating sex differences in social support are somewhat questionable in their ability to predict friendship and relational quality and solidarity. Many other surveys and tests, for instance, find that males actually demonstrate a greater degree of social support than women do in many non-western cultures, particularly from same-gender friendship networks.[15][16][17]
Neuroendocrine underpinnings
Human and animal studies (reviewed in Taylor et al., 2000) suggest that oxytocin is the neuroendocrine mechanism underlying the female "befriend" stress response.[1] Oxytocin administration to rats and prairie voles increased social contact and social grooming behaviors, reduced stress, and lowered aggression. In humans, oxytocin promotes mother-infant attachments, romantic pair bonds, and friendships. Social contact or support during stressful times leads to lowered sympathetic and neuroendocrine stress responses. Although social support downregulates these physiological stress responses in both men and women, women are more likely to seek some forms of social contact during stress. Furthermore, support from another female provides enhanced stress-reducing benefits to women.[18] However, a review of female aggression noted that "The fact that OT [oxytocin] enhances, rather than diminishes, attention to potential threat in the environment casts doubt on the popular ‘tend-and-befriend’ hypothesis which is based on the presumed anxiolytic effect of OT".[19]
Benefits of affiliation under stress
According to Taylor (2000), affiliative behaviors and tending activities reduce biological stress responses in both parents and offspring, thereby reducing stress-related health threats.[20] "Befriending" may lead to substantial mental and physical health benefits in times of stress. Social isolation is associated with significantly enhanced risk of mortality, whereas social support is tied to positive health outcomes, including reduced risk of illness and death.[21]
Women have higher life expectancies from birth in modernized countries where there is equal access to medical care.[22] In the United States, for example, this difference is almost 6 years. One hypothesis is that men's responses to stress (which include aggression, social withdrawal, and substance abuse) place them at risk for adverse health-related consequences.[23] In contrast, women's responses to stress, which include turning to social sources for support, may be protective to health. There are a number of problems and controversies inherent in this reading, however. One major issue is that the female advantage in life expectancy is relatively recent and seems to be related to major societal changes accompanying industrialization, only some of which relate to modern medical advancements.[24] Prior to the Industrial Revolution, men outlived women in many of the societies for which we have demographic data, and in many non-western societies the gap only begun to close and then reverse in the mid-to-late 20th century. The supposed "universality" of women's more adaptive coping mechanisms in response to stress is further challenged by pre-modern data indicating that female rates of suicide were much higher than male rates in many traditional societies.[25][26][27][28][29]
Competition for resources
Group living and affiliation with multiple unrelated others of the same sex (who do not share genetic interests) also presents the problem of competing for access to limited resources, such as social status, food, and mates. Interpersonal stress is the most common and distressing type of stress for women.[30] Although the befriending stress response may be especially activated for women under conditions of resource scarcity,[1] resource scarcity also entails more intense competition for these resources. In environments with a female-biased sex ratio, where males are a more limited resource, female-to-female competition for mates is intensified, sometimes even resorting to violence.[31] Although male crime rates far exceed those of females, arrests for assault among females follow a similar age distribution as in males, peaking for females in the late teens to mid-twenties[citation needed]. Those are ages in which females are at peak reproductive potential and experience the most mating competition. However, the benefits of affiliation would have outweighed the costs in order for tend-and-befriend to have evolved.
Competition and aggression
Rates of aggression between human males and females may not differ, but the patterns of aggression between the sexes do differ in many societies and by many different metrics. Although females in general are less physically aggressive, they tend to engage in as much or even more indirect aggression (e.g. social exclusion, gossip, rumors, denigration).[32] When experimentally primed with a mating motive or status competition motive, men were more willing to become directly aggressive towards another man, whereas women were more likely to indirectly aggress against another woman in an aggression-provoking situation.[33] However, experimentally priming people with a resource competition motive increased direct aggression in both men and women[citation needed]. Consistent with this result, rates of violence and crime are higher among males and females under conditions of resource scarcity.[34] In contrast, resource competition did not increase direct aggression in either men or women when they were asked to imagine themselves married and with a young child[citation needed]. The costs of physical injury to a parent would also entail costs to his or her family.
Lower variance in reproductive success and higher costs of physical aggression may explain the lower rates of physical aggression among human females compared to males.[34] Females are in general more likely to produce offspring in their lifetimes than males, although this difference lessens or disappears in societies where monogamy or polyandry have become standardized. Therefore, they typically have less to gain from fighting and the risk of injury or death would produce greater fitness cost for females. The survival of young children might depend more on maternal than paternal care (although a number of studies of traditional societies have found that parental care in general is less essential than sometimes believed, and can be compensated for via alloparenting by both sexes[35]), which underscores the importance of maternal safety, survival, and risk aversion.[34] In this hypothetical model, infants' primary attachment is to their mother; notably, one study found that maternal death increased the chances of childhood mortality in foraging societies by fivefold, compared to threefold in the cases of paternal death.[34] Therefore, women are believed by certain researchers to respond to threats by tending and befriending, and female aggression is often indirect and covert in nature to avoid retaliation and physical injury.
Informational warfare
Women befriend others not only for protection, but also to form alliances to compete with outgroup members for resources, such as food, mates, and social and cultural resources (e.g. status, social positions, rights and responsibilities). Informational warfare is the strategic, competitive tactics taking the form of indirect, verbal aggression directed towards rivals.[citation needed]Gossip is one such tactic, functioning to spread information that would damage the reputation of a competitor. There are several theories regarding gossip, including social bonding and group cohesion. However, consistent with informational warfare theory, the content of gossip is relevant to the context in which competition is occurring. For example, when competing for a work promotion, people were more likely to spread negative work-related information about a competitor to coworkers.[citation needed] Negative gossip also increases with resource scarcity and higher resource value. In addition, people are more likely to spread negative information about potential rivals but more likely to pass on positive information about family members and friends.
As mentioned above, befriending can serve to protect women from threats, including harm from other people. Such threats are not limited to physical harm but also include reputational damage. Women form friendships and alliances in part to compete for limited resources, and also in part to protect themselves from relational and reputational harm. The presence of friends and allies can help deter malicious gossip, due to an alliance's greater ability to retaliate, compared to a single individual's ability. Studies by Hess and Hagen (2009) show that the presence of a competitor's friend reduced people's tendencies to gossip about the competitor.[36] This effect was stronger when the friend was from the same competitive social environment (e.g. same workplace) than when the friend was from a nonrelevant social environment. Friends increase women's perceived capabilities for inflicting reputational harm on a rival as well as perceptions of defensive capabilities against indirect aggression.
Criticism and controversy
Like most evolutionary psychological theories related to sex differences in behavior, the "tend and befriend" model relies on a great deal of speculation, projection of present-day data into the distant past, untestable and unfalsifiable hypotheses, and reliance on a model of gender essentialism which has come under increasing critique from various social scientists in recent years.
One major issue from an anthropological standpoint is the considerable diversity of gendered norms and behaviors in traditional societies, and the difficulty for western researchers to interpret these adequately using quantitative and etic means. Social and behavioral scientists often struggle to keep their personal biases and paradigms from affecting their interpretation of the data, with mixed results. For instance, anthropologists working within a psychoanalytic framework often set out on their project expecting to find cross-cultural confirmation of western gendered ideas such as castration anxiety or the Oedipus complex, only to run into considerable difficulty when non-western societies frequently deviate from these perceived "universal" norms.[37][38] Sociobiologists and evolutionary psychologists in general have come under fire for cherry-picking and misinterpreting cross-cultural data in order to align with preconceptions about the universality of "human nature", and then accusing cultural anthropologists of various cognitive biases and over-reliance on the alleged "standard social science model".[39][40] The perceived cross-cultural validation of gender norms such as higher female nurturance or male aggression and assertiveness would therefore have to be evaluated, as much as possible, using emic or culturally-specific means, or through researchers trained in culturally sensitive methodologies (such as Franz Boas' cultural relativism) with the hopes of minimizing western cultural biases.
In spite of the perceived universal and biological basis for the tend and befriend response in human women, there is actually a great deal of controversy as to how consistently replicable western gender norms are across the broad range of human societies. Some researchers have found apparently consistent differences across countries favoring women's greater sociability and agreeableness (the dimensions most likely to map onto the tend and befriend theory).[41] However, there are considerable variations between countries, particularly on extraversion, which would seem to frustrate any attempt to find universal bidirectional patterns favoring women's greater tendencies towards cooperative or gregarious behaviors. Many cross-cultural quantitative samples utilized by evolutionary psychologists are also plagued by a patrilineal or patriarchal bias. There is a rich body of data illustrating greater tendencies among women in various cultures toward cooperation, less overt competitiveness, more pro-social and nurturant responses, and preference for indirect and non-confrontational speech styles. For instance, Whiting and Whiting's influential "six culture study" found apparently consistent confirmation of western-stereotyped gender behaviors in six different communities spread across the world: New Englanders in the United States, Mixtec in Mexico, Ilocanos in the Philippines, Rajputs in India, Okinawans in Japan, and Gusii in Kenya.[42] All of these communities are traditional patriarchal, and four of the six are also patrilineal. Even in the two non-patrilineal societies (New Englanders and Ilocanos), there was considerable inculcation towards conformity with patriarchal gender norms, from the capitalistic wage economy in New England and the influence of Spanish Roman Catholicism in the Philippines. This is important since matrilineal and bilateral descent is consistently associated with elimination or even reversal of purported gender differences in competitiveness versus co-operation.[43][44][45] Folklore illustrates another piece of evidence for the diversity of gendered behavioral norms; while the familiar construction of dominant and assertive males vs submissive and nurturing females is replicated frequently in cross-cultural folklore motifs, there are notable exceptions and instances of reversed motifs (dominant and assertive females, submissive and nurturing males) in monogamous or matrilineal cultures like the Kadiweu and the Palikur.[46]
Heide Gottner-Abendroth's analysis of matriarchal societies (which she defines as all non-patriarchal societies) further challenges the notion that men are inherently less nurturant and therefore less prone to tending and befriending.[47] In non-patriarchal societies, men are often expected to internalize virtues that western society codes as stereotypically "feminine", and the culturally constructed machismo which prevents men in many parts of the world from participating in child care or nurturing warm and pro-social coalitional relationships does not seem to exist.[48]
The tend and befriend model also assumes a lower emotional and psychological quality to male same-sex friendships as compared to those between women, interpreting the former as largely "instrumental" and focused on giving and returning favors, building coalitions or acquiring resources while the latter function as superior means of social support. This claim runs squarely counter to data finding that male friendships are equally if not more valuable to men's psychological well-being and societal adjustment than women's.[49][50] This tendency to read men's homosocial relationships as somehow inherently "defective" in terms of psychoemotional support compared to women's does not fit with historical or cross-cultural accounts of deep romantic friendships between males and considerable emotional intimacy that male friends exchange in a number of non-western societies. Even in modern times, some quantitative research suggest that in some societies which are not affected by Northern European male anxieties about homosocial intimacy (such as Turkey or Portugal), men are equally or even more likely than women to share emotional hardships with same-sexed friends and to offer and receive emotional support from them. [51] In the past, before globalization and industrialization standardized the modern cultural traits of males disproportionately "projecting inward" by killing themselves or using maladaptive coping mechanisms (such as substance abuse), such homosocial intimacy may have been higher across much of the world. it's worth noting that in eastern societies where heterosexual cross-sex contact is often limited men display as much intimacy in their same sex friendships and self disclose to their same sex friends just as much if not slightly more such as in India [52] and Jordan [53]
^ abcdefghTaylor, Shelley E.; Klein, Laura Cousino; Lewis, Brian P.; Gruenewald, Tara L.; Gurung, Regan A. R.; Updegraff, John A. (2000). "Biobehavioral responses to stress in females: Tend-and-befriend, not fight-or-flight". Psychological Review. 107 (3): 411–29. CiteSeerX10.1.1.386.912. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.107.3.411. PMID10941275.
^Porges, S. W. (2001). "The Polyvagal Theory: Phylogenetic substrates of a social nervous system". International Journal of Psychophysiology. 42 (2): 123–146. doi:10.1016/s0167-8760(01)00162-3. PMID11587772.
^ abCarter, C.S., Lederhendler, I.I., & Kirkpatrick, B., eds. (1999). The integrative neurobiology of affiliation. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.[page needed]
^Insel, Thomas R. (1997). "A Neurobiological Basis of Social Attachment". American Journal of Psychiatry. 154 (6): 726–35. doi:10.1176/ajp.154.6.726. PMID9167498.
^Light, Kathleen C.; Smith, Tara E.; Johns, Josephine M.; Brownley, Kimberly A.; Hofheimer, Julie A.; Amico, Janet A. (2000). "Oxytocin responsivity in mothers of infants: A preliminary study of relationships with blood pressure during laboratory stress and normal ambulatory activity". Health Psychology. 19 (6): 560–7. doi:10.1037/0278-6133.19.6.560. PMID11129359.
^McCarthy, MM (1995). Estrogen modulation of oxytocin and its relation to behavior. Advances in Experimental Medicine and Biology. Vol. 395. pp. 235–45. PMID8713972.
^Repetti, R. L. (1989). "Effects of daily workload on subsequent behavior during marital interactions: The role of social withdrawal and spouse support". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 57 (4): 651–659. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.57.4.651. PMID2795436.
^Gunnar, M. R.; Gonzales, C. A.; Goodlin, B. L.; Levine, S. (1981). "Behavioral and pituitary-adrenal responses during a prolonged separation period in rhesus monkeys". Psychoneuroimmunology. 6 (1): 65–75. doi:10.1016/0306-4530(81)90049-4. PMID7195597. S2CID20729964.
^Kendrick, K. M.; Keverne, E. B.; Baldwin, B. A. (1987). "Intracerebroventricular oxytocin stimulates maternal behaviour in the sheep". Neuroendocrinology. 46 (1): 56–61. doi:10.1159/000124796. PMID3614555.
^ abBurkart, J. M.; Hrdy, S. B.; Van Schaik, C. P. (September 2009). "Cooperative breeding and human cognitive evolution". Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews. 18 (5): 175–186. CiteSeerX10.1.1.724.8494. doi:10.1002/evan.20222. S2CID31180845.
^Tamres, Lisa K.; Janicki, Denise; Helgeson, Vicki S. (2002). "Sex Differences in Coping Behavior: A Meta-Analytic Review and an Examination of Relative Coping". Personality and Social Psychology Review. 6: 2–30. doi:10.1207/S15327957PSPR0601_1. S2CID144326879.
^Moffatt, M., Schlegel, A., & Barry, H. (1992). Adolescence: an Anthropological Inquiry. Contemporary Sociology, 21(3), 367. https://doi.org/10.2307/2076290
^Gerin, W.; Milner, D.; Chawla, S.; et al. (1995). "Social support as a moderator of cardiovascular reactivity: A test of the direct effects and buffering hypothesis". Psychosomatic Medicine. 57 (1): 16–22. doi:10.1097/00006842-199501000-00003. PMID7732154. S2CID31373563.
^Verbrugge, Lois M. (1985). "Gender and Health: An Update on Hypotheses and Evidence". Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 26 (3): 156–82. doi:10.2307/2136750. JSTOR2136750. PMID3905939.
^Bullough, V., & Campbell, C. (1980). Female longevity and diet in the middle Ages. Speculum, 55(2), 317–325. https://doi.org/10.2307/2847291
^Goullart, P. (2017). Forgotten kingdom: Nine years in Yunnan 1939-48. Eland Publishing.
^Miner, J. R. (1922). Suicide and its relation to climatic and other factors. American journal of hygiene.
^Clark, J. (1980). Hunza, Lost Kingdom of the Himalayas. Indus Publ.
^Reclus, Elisee (1882). "The Earth and Its Inhabitants ...: The north-east Atlantic, islands of the north Atlantic, Scandinavia, European islands of the Arctic ocean, Russia in Europe."
^Campbell, A. (1995). "A few good men: Evolutionary psychology and female adolescent aggression". Ethology and Sociobiology. 16 (2): 99–123. doi:10.1016/0162-3095(94)00072-f.
^Bjorkqvist, K.; Niemela, P., eds. (1992). Of mice and women: Aspects of female aggression. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
^Griskevicius, Vladas; et al. (2009). "Aggress to Impress: Hostility as an Evolved Context-Dependent Strategy". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 96 (5): 980–994. doi:10.1037/a0013907. PMID19379031.
^Wood, W., & Eagly, A. H. (2002). A cross-cultural analysis of the behavior of women and men: Implications for the origins of sex differences. Psychological Bulletin, 128(5), 699–727. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.128.5.699
^Seligman, C. G., Malinowski, B., & Ellis, H. (1929). The Sexual Life of Savages in North-Western Melanesia: An Ethnographic Account of Courtship, Marriage, and Family Life among the Natives of the Trobriand Islands, British New Guinea. Economica, 25, 86. https://doi.org/10.2307/2548523
^Dreger, A. (2011). The Trashing of Margaret Mead: Anatomy of an anthropological controversy. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 40(6), 1341–1343. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-011-9843-0
^Schmitt, D. P., Realo, A., Voracek, M., & Allik, J. (2008). Why can't a man be more like a woman? Sex differences in Big Five personality traits across 55 cultures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94(1), 168–182. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.94.1.168
^Gneezy, Uri, Kenneth L Leonard and John A List (2009), “Gender differences in competition: Evidence from a matrilineal and a patriarchal society”, Econometrica, 77 (5): 1637–1664
^Ji, T., Wu, J., He, Q., Xu, J., Mace, R., & Tao, Y. (2013). Reproductive competition between females in the matrilineal Mosuo of southwestern China. Philosophical Transactions - Royal Society. Biological Sciences, 368(1631), 20130081. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2013.0081
Kurfürstentum Trier (de) Électorat de Trèves (fr) Land in het Heilige Roomse Rijk 902 – 1803 → → Symbolen (Details) Kaart 1648 Algemene gegevens Hoofdstad Trier, Koblenz Religie Rooms-katholicisme Politieke gegevens Regeringsvorm Geestelijk Rijksvorstendom Staatshoofd Prins-aartsbisschop Kreits Keur-Rijnse KreitsBoven-Rijnse Kreits (Prüm) De Rococovleugel van het Keurvorstelijk Paleis (roze), zetel van het aartsbisdom tot 1794, en de Romeinse Basilica van Constantijn Het ...
У этого термина существуют и другие значения, см. Деснянский район. Деснянский районукр. Деснянський район Флаг Герб Киев Дата основания 1987 год Прежние имена Ватутинский район Первое упоминание 2001 Площадь 154,2 км² Население (2022) ▼364 887[1] чел. Плотность населения 23...
Constituency of the National Assembly of France 2nd constituency of Tarn-et-GaronneinlineConstituency of the National Assembly of FranceTarn-et-Garonne's 2nd Constituency shown within the Tarn-et-GaronneDeputyMarine HameletRNDepartmentTarn-et-GaronneCantonsAuvillar, Beaumont-de-Lomagne, Bourg-de-Visa, Castelsarrasin I, Castelsarrasin II, Grisolles, Lauzerte, Lavit, Moissac I, Moissac II, Montech, Montaigu-de-Quercy, Saint-Nicolas-de-la-Grave, Valence, Verdun-sur-GaronneRegistered voters93,323...
«End Game» Episodio de The X-FilesTítulo traducido «El juego final» (Hispanoamérica)«Fin de juego» (España)Episodio n.º Temporada 2Episodio 17Dirigido por Rob BowmanEscrito por Frank SpotnitzGuion por Frank SpotnitzCód. de producción 2X17[1]Duración 45 minutosEmisión 17 de febrero de 1995Estrella(s) invitada(s) Steven Williams como X Peter Donat como William Mulder Brian Thompson como el Cazarrecompensas extraterrestre Megan Leitch como Falsa Samantha Mulde...
У Вікіпедії є статті про інші географічні об’єкти з назвою Тейлорсвілл. Місто Тейлорсвіллангл. Taylorsville Файл:Tville arial.jpg Координати 31°49′48″ пн. ш. 89°25′49″ зх. д. / 31.83000000002777696° пн. ш. 89.43030000002778479° зх. д. / 31.83000000002777696; -89.43030000002778479Координати: 31°49′48...
Artikel ini sebatang kara, artinya tidak ada artikel lain yang memiliki pranala balik ke halaman ini.Bantulah menambah pranala ke artikel ini dari artikel yang berhubungan atau coba peralatan pencari pranala.Tag ini diberikan pada November 2022. Useless HumansSutradara Stephen Ohl Produser Ryan Scaringe Jon Bloch Ditulis oleh Stephen Ohl George Caine Kevin Hamedani Travis Betz Ryan Scaringe PemeranJosh ZuckermanDavida WilliamsLuke YoungbloodRushi KotaEdy GanemMaya KazanJoey KernIqbal ThebaTim...
This article has multiple issues. Please help improve it or discuss these issues on the talk page. (Learn how and when to remove these template messages) This article needs additional citations for verification. Please help improve this article by adding citations to reliable sources. Unsourced material may be challenged and removed.Find sources: Prince William County Sheriff's Office – news · newspapers · books · scholar · JSTOR (December 2021) (Learn...
Demographics of Papua New GuineaPopulation pyramid of Papua New Guinea in 2020Population9,593,498 (2022 est.)Growth rate2.35% (2022 est.)Birth rate29.03 births/1,000 population (2022 est.)Death rate5.54 deaths/1,000 population (2022 est.)Life expectancy69.43 yearsFertility rate3.92 children born/woman (2022 est.)Infant mortality rate33.59 deaths/1,000 live birthsNet migration rate0 migrant(s)/1,000 population (2022 est.)NationalityNationalityPapua New Guinean The indigenous population of Papu...
Audi Q9 Общие данные Производитель Audi Годы производства 2022 — настоящее время Класс SUV Дизайн и конструкция Тип кузова 5‑дв. SUV (7‑мест.) Платформа Volkswagen Group MLB Компоновка переднемоторная, полноприводная Колёсная формула 4 × 4 Двигатель Volkswagen EA839 Volkswagen EA825 Массогаб...
This article includes a list of references, related reading, or external links, but its sources remain unclear because it lacks inline citations. Please help to improve this article by introducing more precise citations. (November 2018) (Learn how and when to remove this template message) Motor vehicle Volkswagen LogusOverviewManufacturerVolkswagen, AutolatinaProduction1993–1997DesignerGhiaBody and chassisClassSmall family car (C)Body style2-door coupeLayoutFF layoutRelatedFord Escort ...
Este artículo o sección necesita ser wikificado, por favor, edítalo para que cumpla con las convenciones de estilo.Este aviso fue puesto el 6 de febrero de 2015. Ve Interactive Tipo PrivadaIndustria Tecnología software, E-commerce, Marketing Digital, Publicidad DigitalFundación 2009Fundador David J. Brown/Steve ClucasSede central Old Street, East London Tech City, LondonPersonas clave Morten Tonnesen CEOProductos Soluciones software productos para E-commerce retailersEmpleados ~500Sitio ...
American actor and singer (born 1981) For the album, see Leslie Odom Jr. (album). Leslie Odom Jr.Odom Jr. in 2016BornLeslie Lloyd Odom Jr. (1981-08-06) August 6, 1981 (age 42)New York City, U.S.EducationCarnegie Mellon University (BFA)Occupation(s)Actor, singerYears active1998-presentSpouse Nicolette Robinson (m. 2012)Children2Websiteleslieodomjr.com Leslie Lloyd Odom Jr. (/ˈoʊdəm/; born August 6, 1981)[1] is an American actor and singer. He...
Cet article est une ébauche concernant un barrage. Vous pouvez partager vos connaissances en l’améliorant (comment ?) selon les recommandations des projets correspondants. Barrage d'ArpaçayGéographiePays TurquieProvince KarsCoordonnées 40° 33′ 46″ N, 43° 38′ 43″ ECours d'eau Arpaçay (Akhourian)Objectifs et impactsVocation Irrigation et régulationDate du début des travaux 1980Date de mise en service 1983BarrageType Poids en bétonHau...
Ini adalah nama Batak Toba, marganya adalah Simanjorang. Wilmar Eliaser SimandjorangPenjabat Bupati SamosirMasa jabatan15 Januari 2004 – 13 September 2005GubernurTengku Rizal NurdinRudolf PardedePendahulujabatan baruPenggantiMangindar Simbolon (sebagai Bupati Samosir) Informasi pribadiLahir(1954-10-11)11 Oktober 1954Samosir, Tapanuli Utara, Sumatera UtaraSuami/istriNurhaida Simarmata (m. 1981)Situs webwilmareliasersimandjorang.orgSunting kotak info...
Italian rapper (born 2003) Anna PepeBorn (2003-08-15) 15 August 2003 (age 20)La Spezia, ItalyGenresPop rapInstrument(s)VocalsYears active2018–presentLabelsVirginMusical artist Anna Pepe (born 15 August 2003), known professionally as Anna, is an Italian rapper. In 2020, she became the youngest artist to top the Italian singles chart with Bando, her debut single.[1][2] Biography 2018-2020: Early years In 2018, Anna started to post freestyle rap videos on Instagram, before...
Species of scorpion Cercophonius squama Scientific classification Kingdom: Animalia Phylum: Arthropoda Subphylum: Chelicerata Class: Arachnida Order: Scorpiones Family: Bothriuridae Genus: Cercophonius Species: C. squama Binomial name Cercophonius squama(Gervais, 1844) Cercophonius squama, commonly known as the forest scorpion or wood scorpion, is a scorpion native to south-eastern Australia. It is typically around 25–40 mm (0.98–1.57 in) long.[1] Its colour consist...
22nd season of the Power Rangers franchise broadcast in 2015 This article uses bare URLs, which are uninformative and vulnerable to link rot. Please consider converting them to full citations to ensure the article remains verifiable and maintains a consistent citation style. Several templates and tools are available to assist in formatting, such as reFill (documentation) and Citation bot (documentation). (August 2022) (Learn how and when to remove this template message) Power Rangers Dino Cha...
Aleksandr Yúrievich Sasha Kaleri (Ruso: Александр Юрьевич Калери; Jūrmala, Letonia, 13 de mayo de 1956) es un cosmonauta ruso y veterano de estadías prolongadas en la estación espacial Mir y en la Estación Espacial Internacional. Ha pasado un tiempo en el espacio de 609d, 21h, y 53m. Kalerí tiene una licenciatura del Instituto de Física y Tecnología de Moscú. En 1979, fue contratado por la corporación Energía y trabajó en el diseño de la estación espacial M...
Strategi Solo vs Squad di Free Fire: Cara Menang Mudah!