Serial reaction time

Serial reaction time (SRT) is a commonly used parameter in the measurement of unconscious learning processes.[1] This parameter is operationalised through a SRT task, in which participants are asked to repeatedly respond to a fixed set of stimuli in which each cue signals that a particular response (i.e., button press) needs to be made. Unbeknownst to the participant, there are probabilities governing the occurrence of the cues as they appear in both a repeated sequence and randomised order, thus required responses following one cue have some predictability, influencing reaction-time. As a result, reaction-time to these cues becomes increasingly fast as subjects learn and utilise these transition probabilities. [2]

Combined with artificial grammar learning methods, this paradigm has been used to study a range of learning phenomena including language structure learning, memory,[3][4] and syntactic priming.

History of SRT Task

The conceptualisation of SRT began with researchers Nissen and Bullemer in 1987, who believed a performance based parameter was necessary in the investigation of attentional requirements of learning and memory.[5] The experimental procedure of early studies that investigated attention, awareness and memory (therefore learning) (including Moray's Attention in Dichotic Listening (1959) and Norman's Memory while Shadowing (1969) amongst others) often involved the presentation of a set of stimuli through some form (e.g, verbal repetition in one ear) while the participant would simultaneously engage in another task that was assumed to require additional attention. This would result in them "unattending" to the presented stimuli (often a set of words), to which experiment results mostly demonstrated that recall of the "unattended" stimuli was poor[6]

After thorough analysis of literature (including Moray (1959) and Norman (1969)), Nissen and Bullemer found that the procedures used in measuring recall of the "unattended" stimuli in such studies were highly reliant on traditional introspective measures, such as recall or recognition tests. Developing research at the time however highlighted that memory for an existing or preceding experience can also be reflected in the subsequent performance of a task, not requiring deliberate manipulation of consciousness. Alongside this, memory for implicit cognitive skills like perceptual and motor learning was investigated through more performance-based parameters.[6] In combination, this led to the direction of their research on investigating whether a performance-based measure like the SRT Task would reflect reduced dependency on the allocation of attention on a stimulus over more traditional introspective measures.[5]

SRT Task Procedure

While there have been various adaptations of the SRT task in correspondence to different areas of Psychological research, at its most basic form the task involves a motor-based interaction with a computer in which participants must respond to a change in a visual stimuli appearing on-screen.[1]

Method [1]

Sequence: 1. Visual cue 2. Corresponding button 3. Fixed delay 4. Next visual cue (This image contains five stimuli positions unlike the original task)

In a horizontal arrangement, a visual cue may appear in one of four locations on-screen. Each location on the screen is coded to correspond to a button on a response-pad, with buttons typically represented by keys on a keyboard in modern settings. When the visual cue appears on screen, participants must press the corresponding key (e.g, location 1 corresponds to the numerical key "1" on a keyboard) as fast as possible in response to the cue. Each time that this is done defines the end of a trial, therefore the duration of a trial is determined by the participant's reaction-time in pressing the corresponding key. This is the primary measurement of the task. After each trial, there is a short delay of between 200-500 milliseconds before the next cue is shown, thus starting the next trial round.

The pattern in which the cues are shown each trial is predetermined and intentional, with a specific sequential chain being repeated (for instance, 2-3-1-4-3-2-4-1-3-4-2-1) followed by a few random trials in a non-repeating order. The repeating chain of cues is typically repeated as a set of 10.[7]

SRT Task Applications

There are two distinct ways in which the SRT task demonstrates learning processes, primarily in implicit learning and also motor learning.[1] Learning in either case is shown to take place through the gradual reduction of reaction-time across trials.

This decrease in reaction-time suggests increasing familiarity with the repeated components of the stimuli, as there is an increase in learning of the visuomotor association between the cue's position and ability to respond, therefore serving as a measurement for participants' increase in skill learnt. It is important to note the response modality of the SRT task which records learning through the measurement of visual and sequential efficiency. There are other performance-based implicit learning tasks such as a Contingent Response Task, Hebb Digits Task, Function matching among several others that have greater emphasis on other learning modalities like conceptual fluency, prediction and control.[8]

Implicit learning

As previously referred to, implicit learning refers to the process of acquiring information in an incidental manner, without the requirement of awareness for learning to take place.[8] This is measured through the SRT task given the gradual reduction in participant response time over the course of the repetition of the set of 10 sequential cues, disguised within the randomly occurring trials in between the sequence. Because the repeated sequence is not made aware to participants, their improvement in performance suggests implicit learning taking place.

One of the findings in Nissen and Bullemer's "Experiment 1" of their original study (1987) using the SRT task supports this. Results found that the average reaction-time of participants for the repeating sequence decreased from 327 milliseconds in the first block of 100 trials to 163 milliseconds in a later block, suggesting that the sequences had somewhat been implicitly learnt or recognised in memory, ultimately leading to the reduced reaction-times.[2]

Learning from this task is more evident when contrasting the reaction-times of participants in periods where they are responding to the disguised repeating sequence versus the non-repeating, random trials. When trials of the repeating sequence are replaced by random ones, adapted studies observed that participants initially began to inappropriately respond to the random sequence, leading to increased reaction-times in comparison to when the repeating sequence of cues were being presented.[1]

Motor learning

Motor learning is also found to take place through the SRT task as participants acquire the ability to reproduce the repeated sequence by remembering the order in which buttons are pressed. However, motor learning through the task is not facilitated in isolation but instead the skill of reacting could be learned perceptually with participants basing their next prediction on their memory of the visual cue's previous position.[1] Whether the task facilitates pure motor learning or perceptual learning is still being investigated further through modifications of the task, attempting to study the occurrence of one type of learning in isolation of the other.[9]

References

  1. ^ a b c d e f Robertson, E. M. (2007). "The Serial Reaction Time Task: Implicit Motor Skill Learning?". Journal of Neuroscience. 27 (38): 10073–10075. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2747-07.2007. PMC 6672677. PMID 17881512.
  2. ^ a b Nissen, M. J.; Bullemer, P. (1987). "Attentional requirements of learning: Evidence from performance measures". Cognitive Psychology. 19: 1–32. doi:10.1016/0010-0285(87)90002-8. S2CID 54370482.
  3. ^ Unsworth, Nash; Engle, Randall W. (2005-03-01). "Individual differences in working memory capacity and learning: Evidence from the serial reaction time task". Memory & Cognition. 33 (2): 213–220. doi:10.3758/BF03195310. ISSN 1532-5946. PMID 16028576.
  4. ^ Tamayo, Ricardo; Frensch, Peter A. (2015-01-01). "Temporal Stability of Implicit Sequence Knowledge". Experimental Psychology. 62 (4): 240–253. doi:10.1027/1618-3169/a000293. ISSN 1618-3169. PMID 26138300.
  5. ^ a b Nissen, Mary Jo; Bullemer, Peter (1987). "Attentional requirements of learning: Evidence from performance measures". Cognitive Psychology. 19 (1): 27–30. doi:10.1016/0010-0285(87)90002-8. S2CID 54370482.
  6. ^ a b Nissen, Mary Jo; Bullemer, Peter (1987). "Attentional requirements of learning: Evidence from performance measures". Cognitive Psychology. 19 (1): 1–4. doi:10.1016/0010-0285(87)90002-8. S2CID 54370482.
  7. ^ Seger, Carol Augart (1994). "Implicit learning". Psychological Bulletin. 115 (2): 167. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.115.2.163. ISSN 1939-1455. PMID 8165269.
  8. ^ a b Seger, Carol Augart (1994). "Implicit learning". Psychological Bulletin. 115 (2): 168. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.115.2.163. ISSN 1939-1455. PMID 8165269.
  9. ^ Dennis, Nancy A.; Howard, James H.; Howard, Darlene V. (2006). "Implicit sequence learning without motor sequencing in young and old adults". Experimental Brain Research. 175 (1): 153–164. doi:10.1007/s00221-006-0534-3. ISSN 0014-4819. PMID 16788812. S2CID 4651072.


Read other articles:

Nueva visita a un mundo feliz de Aldous Huxley Género Cuento Subgénero Ensayo Tema(s) Un mundo feliz Idioma Inglés Título original Brave New World Revisited Fecha de publicación 1958 [editar datos en Wikidata] Nueva visita a un mundo feliz es una recopilación de ensayos (publicados en la revista Newsday) sobre la novela Un mundo feliz (1932), de Aldous Huxley, en donde el mismo autor analiza veintiséis años después (1958) los contenidos tratados en su novela, verifican...

 

Ancient Roman settlement Caenophrurium (also written as Cenophrurium and Coenophrurium; Greek: Καινοφρούριον Kainophrurion) was a settlement in the Roman province of Europa (the southeasternmost part of Thrace), between Byzantium and Heraclea Perinthus. It appears in late Roman and early Byzantine accounts. Caenophrurium translates as the stronghold of the Caeni, a Thracian tribe. Location Classical scholars have at times identified various towns in Thrace as corresponding to Cae...

 

2008 film by Damon Santostefano This article needs additional citations for verification. Please help improve this article by adding citations to reliable sources. Unsourced material may be challenged and removed.Find sources: Another Cinderella Story – news · newspapers · books · scholar · JSTOR (March 2019) (Learn how and when to remove this template message) Another Cinderella StoryDVD coverDirected byDamon SantostefanoWritten by Erik Patterson Jess...

 

4654 ГорькавийВідкриттяВідкривач Черних Микола СтепановичМісце відкриття КрАОДата відкриття 11 вересня 1977ПозначенняНазвана на честь Nick GorkavyidТимчасові позначення 1977 RJ6 1984 WK4 1986 HMКатегорія малої планети Астероїд головного поясуОрбітальні характеристики[1] Епоха 23 тр

 

Israel padaOlimpiadeKode IOCISRKONKomite Olimpiade IsraelSitus webwww.olympic.one.co.il (dalam bahasa Ibrani)Medali 2 1 9 Total 12 Penampilan Musim Panas195219561960196419681972197619801984198819921996200020042008201220162020Penampilan Musim Dingin19941998200220062010201420182022 Berikut ini adalah pembawa bendera yang mewakili Israel pada Olimpiade.[1] Para pembawa bendera membawa bendera nasional negara mereka pada upacara-upacara pembukaan dan penutupan Permainan Oli...

 

Este artículo o sección necesita referencias que aparezcan en una publicación acreditada.Este aviso fue puesto el 23 de noviembre de 2014. Portpatrick Entidad subnacional PortpatrickLocalización de Portpatrick en EscociaCoordenadas 54°50′42″N 5°07′27″O / 54.845, -5.12429Entidad Pueblo • País  Reino UnidoPoblación (2016)   • Total 640 hab.Huso horario UTC±00:00Código postal DG9Prefijo telefónico 01776-81[editar datos en ...

 

c. 1520 painting by Titian The Entombment of ChristArtistTitianYearc. 1520[1]MediumOil on canvasDimensions148 cm × 212 cm (58 in × 83 in)LocationMusée du Louvre, Paris The Entombment of Christ is a c. 1520[1] painting by Titian, now in the Musée du Louvre, in Paris. History The work was once in the collection of the Gonzaga family, before it was bought in 1627 by Charles I of England. When Charles died, his art was put ...

 

Artikel ini tidak memiliki referensi atau sumber tepercaya sehingga isinya tidak bisa dipastikan. Tolong bantu perbaiki artikel ini dengan menambahkan referensi yang layak. Tulisan tanpa sumber dapat dipertanyakan dan dihapus sewaktu-waktu.Cari sumber: Daftar Stasiun Kereta Api di Kabupaten Serang – berita · surat kabar · buku · cendekiawan · JSTORBerikut ini adalah daftar stasiun di Kabupaten Serang, Banten yang telah terdaftar di Dishub Banten: No. N...

 

هذه المقالة يتيمة إذ تصل إليها مقالات أخرى قليلة جدًا. فضلًا، ساعد بإضافة وصلة إليها في مقالات متعلقة بها. (يوليو 2019) لاري فرايداي معلومات شخصية الميلاد 23 يناير 1958 (65 سنة)  جاكسون، مسيسيبي  مواطنة الولايات المتحدة  الطول 76 بوصة  الوزن 215 رطل  الحياة العملية المهنة...

 

العلاقات البحرينية النمساوية البحرين النمسا   البحرين   النمسا تعديل مصدري - تعديل   العلاقات البحرينية النمساوية هي العلاقات الثنائية التي تجمع بين البحرين والنمسا.[1][2][3][4][5] مقارنة بين البلدين هذه مقارنة عامة ومرجعية للدولتين: وجه المقا�...

 

Code editor for the AppleScript and Javascript for Automation scripting languages This article needs additional citations for verification. Please help improve this article by adding citations to reliable sources. Unsourced material may be challenged and removed.Find sources: AppleScript Editor – news · newspapers · books · scholar · JSTOR (November 2018) (Learn how and when to remove this template message) Script EditorScript Editor running on macOS M...

 

Namaku MentariGenre Drama Roman Remaja PembuatSinemArtSutradaraWidi WijayaPemeran Amanda Jessica Anastasia Bobby Joseph Kevin Julio Stuart Collin Ciccio Manassero Febby Rastanty Gisela Cindy Yadi Timo Rudi Salam Della Puspita Umar Lubis Penggubah lagu temaNidjiLagu pembukaJangan Lupakan — NidjiLagu penutupJangan Lupakan — NidjiNegara asalIndonesiaBahasa asliBahasa IndonesiaJmlh. musim1Jmlh. episode117ProduksiProduser eksekutifElly Yanti NoorProduserLeo SutantoPengaturan kameraMulti-kamera...

 

УНІАН(Українське Незалежне Інформаційне Агентство Новин) Tipo agencia de noticias y sitio webIndustria Agencia de informaciónFundación Marzo de 1993Sede central Khreshchatyk, Kiev, UcraniaPropietario 1+1Empresa matriz 1+1Coordenadas 50°27′09″N 30°31′35″E / 50.45238876, 30.52647209Sitio web www.unian.ua[editar datos en Wikidata] La Agencia de Información Independiente Ucraniana (en ucraniano: Україн�...

 

Carmel Mountain Ranch Barrio de San Diego Coordenadas 32°58′40″N 117°04′45″O / 32.977853, -117.079239Entidad Barrio de San Diego • País  Estados Unidos[editar datos en Wikidata] Carmel Mountain Ranch es una comunidad en el nordeste de San Diego, California. Carmel Mountain Ranch está localizado al oeste de Poway, al sur de Rancho Bernardo, al este de Rancho Peñasquitos, y al norte de Sabre Springs. Carmel Mountain Ranch es comúnmente conocid...

 

Istana Iskandariah Istana Iskandariah adalah istana negara negeri Perak. Istana ini digunakan untuk kegiatan upacara resmi kenegaraan. Pembangunan Istana Iskandariah dimulai tahun 1930. Istana ini selesai dibangun pada tahun 1933. Gaya arsitekturnya merupakan perpaduan dari beberapa kebudayaan. Corak yang berpengaruh ialah arsitektur Islam dengan pengaruh arsitektur India masa saracen. Gaya arsitektur ini kemudian dipadukan dengan gaya arsitektur Asia Barat. Ciri khas Istana Iskandariah adala...

 

American film director For the explorer, see Richard Maurice (explorer). Richard D. MauriceRichard D. Maurice in a scene from the film Eleven P.M.Born(1893-06-14)June 14, 1893Matanzas, CubaDiedFebruary 5, 1955(1955-02-05) (aged 61)[1]New York City[1]Occupation(s)Film director, union organizerNotable workEleven P.M.Spouse(s)Vivian Irene Birdie I. MadisonChildrenRichard D. Maurice, Jr. (b. 1916)Wanda Irene Maurice (b. 1923) Richard D. Maurice (June 14, 1893 – February 5, ...

 

Railway station in Izunokuni, Shizuoka Prefecture, Japan Baraki Station原木駅Baraki Station in November 2007General informationLocationBaraki 763-3, Izunokuni City, Shizuoka PrefectureJapanCoordinates35°03′55.68″N 138°56′37.04″E / 35.0654667°N 138.9436222°E / 35.0654667; 138.9436222Operated by Izuhakone RailwayLine(s)     Sunzu LineDistance8.5 km (5.3 mi) from MishimaPlatforms2 side platformsTracks2ConstructionStructure...

 

This article does not cite any sources. Please help improve this article by adding citations to reliable sources. Unsourced material may be challenged and removed.Find sources: Sky Box Sets – news · newspapers · books · scholar · JSTOR (August 2017) (Learn how and when to remove this template message) Television channel Sky Box SetsCountryNew ZealandBroadcast areaNationalProgrammingPicture format1080i (HDTV) 16:9OwnershipOwnerSky Network TelevisionHist...

 

Gisela Dulkoісп. Gisela Dulko Громадянство  АргентинаМісце проживання Буенос-Айрес, АргентинаДата народження 30 січня 1985(1985-01-30)[1][2] (38 років)Місце народження Буенос-Айрес, АргентинаЗріст 170 смВага 56 кгПочаток кар'єри 2001Робоча рука праваБекхенд дворучнийПризові, USD 3 67...

 

Sri Lankan politician Hon.Chandrasena WljesingheMPMember of Parliamentfor Galle DistrictIn office2004–2010 Personal detailsPolitical partyJanatha Vimukthi Peramuna Chandrasena Wljesinghe is a Sri Lankan politician and a former member of the Parliament of Sri Lanka. He lost a re-election bid in 2010.[1] References ^ Historic win for UPFA. The Island Online. Retrieved 29 April 2011. vte← Members of the 13th Parliament of Sri Lanka (2004 (2004)–2010) → Speaker: W. J. M. Loku...