Human multitasking is the concept that one can split their attention on more than one task or activity at the same time, such as speaking on the phone while driving a car.
Multitasking can result in time wasted due to human context switching (e.g., determining which step is next in the task just switched to) and becoming prone to errors due to insufficient attention. Some people may be proficient at the tasks in question and also be able to rapidly shift attention between the tasks, and therefore perform the tasks well; however, self-perception of being good at multitasking or getting more done while multitasking is frequently inaccurate.[1][2]
Multitasking is mentally and physically stressful for everyone,[3] to the point that multitasking is used in laboratory experiments to study stressful environments.[4] Research suggests that people who are multitasking in a learning environment are worse at learning new information compared to those who do not have their attention divided among different tasks.[5][6][7]
The first published use of the word "multitask" appeared in an IBM paper describing the capabilities of the IBM System/360 in 1965.[8] The term has since been applied to human tasks.
Research
Since the 1960s, psychologists have conducted experiments on the nature and limits of human multitasking. The simplest experimental design used to investigate human multitasking is the so-called psychological refractory period effect. Here, people are asked to make separate responses to each of two stimuli presented close together in time. An extremely general finding is a slowing in responses to the second-appearing stimulus.[1]
Researchers have long suggested that there appears to be a processing bottleneck preventing the brain from working on certain key aspects of both tasks at the same time[9] (e.g., (Gladstones, Regan & Lee 1989) (Pashler 1994)). Bottlenecking refers to the idea that because people only have a limited amount of attentional resources, the most important information is kept. Many researchers believe that the cognitive function subject to the most severe form of bottlenecking is the planning of actions and retrieval of information from memory.[10] Psychiatrist Edward M. Hallowell[11] has gone so far as to describe multitasking as a "mythical activity in which people believe they can perform two or more tasks simultaneously as effectively as one."
Others have researched multitasking in the area of learning. Richard E Mayer and Moreno studied the phenomenon of cognitive load in multimedia learning and concluded that it is difficult, if not impossible, to learn new information while engaging in multitasking.[5]Reynol Junco and Shelia R CottenArchived August 1, 2020, at the Wayback Machine examined how multitasking affects academic success and found that students who engaged in high levels of multitasking reported significant issues with their academic work.[12] A more recent[when?] study on the effects of multitasking on academic performance showed that using Facebook and text messaging while studying were negatively related to student grades, while online searching and emailing were not.[citation needed]
Some experiments have been done that demonstrate that it is possible to divide one's attention among several tasks, how successfully depends on several factors such as how much practice one has with it or the difficulty of the task. Walter Schneider and Robert Shiffrin performed an experiment in which they presented the participants with a memory set, which consists of target stimuli such as the number three. After being presented with the memory set they were rapidly shown 20 test frames which contained distractor stimuli. One of the slides they were shown contained one of the target stimuli from the memory set. With each trial, a new memory set and new test frames were presented. At the start of the experiment, participants averaged 55% in correctly identifying the target stimuli from the memory set. After 900 trials the participants were able to bring the average up to 90%. They reported that after about 600 trials the task became automatic and they were able to respond without thinking about it.[13]
The brain's role
Because the brain cannot fully focus when multitasking, people take longer to complete tasks and are predisposed to error. When people attempt to complete many tasks at one time, “or [alternate] rapidly between them, errors go way up, and it takes far longer—often double the time or more—to get the jobs done than if they were done sequentially,” states Meyer.[2] This is largely because "the brain is compelled to restart and refocus".[14] A study by Meyer and David Kieras found that in the interim between each exchange, the brain makes no progress whatsoever. Therefore, multitasking people not only perform each task less suitably, but lose time in the process.
According to a study done by Jordan Grafman, chief of the cognitive neuroscience section at the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke, "the most anterior part [of the brain] allows [a person] to leave something when it's incomplete and return to the same place and continue from there," while Brodmann Area 10, a part of the brain's frontal lobes, is important for establishing and attaining long-term goals.[2] Focusing on multiple dissimilar tasks at once forces the brain to process all activity in its anterior. Though the brain is complex and can perform myriad tasks, it cannot multitask well.
Another study by René Marois, a psychologist at Vanderbilt University, discovered that the brain exhibits a "response selection bottleneck" when asked to perform several tasks at once. The brain must then decide which activity is most important, thereby taking more time. Psychologist David Meyer, of the University of Michigan, claims that instead of a "bottleneck," the brain experiences "adaptive executive control" which places priorities on each activity. These viewpoints differ in that while bottlenecking attempts to force many thoughts through the brain at once, adaptive executive control prioritizes tasks to maintain a semblance of order. The brain better understands this order and, as psychologists such as Dr. Meyer believe, can, therefore, be trained to multitask.[15] It is not known exactly how the brain processes input and reacts to overstimulation.
Some research suggests that the human brain can be trained to multitask. A study published in Child Development by Monica Luciana, associate professor of psychology at the University of Minnesota, discovered that the brain's capability of categorizing competing information continues to develop until ages sixteen and seventeen. A study by Vanderbilt University found that multitasking is largely limited by "the speed with which our prefrontal cortex processes information." Paul E. Dux, the co-author of the study, believes that this process can become faster through proper training. The study trained seven people to perform two simple tasks, either separately or together, and conducted brain scans of the participants. The individuals multitasked poorly at first but, with training, were able to adeptly perform the tasks simultaneously. Brain scans of the participants indicate that the prefrontal cortex quickened its ability to process the information, enabling the individuals to multitask more efficiently. However, the study also suggests that the brain is incapable of performing multiple tasks at one time, even after extensive training.[16] This study further indicates that, while the brain can become adept at processing and responding to certain information, it cannot truly multitask.
People have a limited ability to retain information, which worsens when the amount of information increases. For this reason, people alter information to make it more memorable, such as separating a ten-digit phone number into three smaller groups or dividing the alphabet into sets of three to five letters, a phenomenon known as chunking. George Miller, former psychologist at Harvard University, believes the limits to the human brain's capacity centers around "the number seven, plus or minus two." An illustrative example of this is a test in which a person must repeat numbers read aloud. While two or three numbers are easily repeated, fifteen numbers become more difficult. The person would, on average, repeat seven correctly.[17] Brains are only capable of storing a limited amount of information in their short-term memories.
Laboratory-based studies of multi-tasking indicate that one motivation for switching between tasks is to increase the time spent on the task that produces the most reward (Payne, Duggan & Neth, 2007). This reward could be progress towards an overall task goal, or it could simply be the opportunity to pursue a more interesting or fun activity. Payne, Duggan, and Neth (2007) found that decisions to switch task reflected either the reward provided by the current task or the availability of a suitable opportunity to switch (i.e. the completion of a subgoal). A French fMRI study published in 2010 indicated preliminary support for the hypothesis that the brain can pursue at most two goals simultaneously, one for each frontal lobe (which has a goal-oriented area).[18]
When studying the costs of multitasking there are typically two designs for or types of multitasking that are examined, task switching and dual tasking. Task switching involves shifting one’s attention from one thing to another. Dual tasking, on the other hand, is when attention is divided among multiple things at once. Studies have been done to specifically examine the brain when one is engaged in either type of multitasking. Through the use of MRI brain scans, researchers have found that frontoparietal regions are activated which would include the inferior frontal junction and the posterior parietal cortex.[19][20] They also found that while each type of tasking uses different mechanisms there are also some underlying mechanisms and resources that they share.[21]
Sex differences
Although some cultures believe that women are better at multitasking than men, there is little data available to support claims of a real sex difference. Most studies that do show any sex differences tend to find that the differences are small and inconsistent.[22]
In 2013, a brain connectivity study from Penn Medicine found major differences in men and women's neural wiring that researchers suggested indicated that sex plays a role in multitasking skills. They said that "[On] average, men are more likely better at learning and performing a single task at hand, like cycling or navigating directions, whereas women have superior memory and social cognition skills, making them more equipped for multitasking and creating solutions that work for a group."[23][24] However, this study has been widely criticized because the differences could easily have been caused by increased head movement. Moreover, the link between the DTI data and behavioral performance is speculative.[citation needed]
In 2018, a study in Norway tested everyday scenarios via videogames and found that "none of the multitasking measures (accuracy, total time, total distance covered by the avatar, a prospective memory score, and a distractor management score) showed any sex differences."[25]
A 2019 study showed that there are not significant sex differences in multi-tasking across numerous tasks.[26]
There have been attempts to produce evolutionary explanations for the popular belief.[27][28][29] One story told by evolutionary biologists Silverman and Eals speculated that a sex-based division of labor into hunters and gatherers could favor the development of a difference in men and women's cognitive abilities, based on the hunter-gatherer tasks each sex performed in the prehistoric past. This is based on the outdated[30] belief that prehistoric males were hunters, while women were gatherers and took care of the children, and that over time, there was a natural selection for women who could multitask, resulting in modern females being superior multitaskers.[31] However, an attempted verification of this study found "that multiple methodological failures all bias their results in the same direction...their analysis does not contradict the wide body of empirical evidence for gendered divisions of labor in foraging societies".[32]
Author Steven Berlin Johnson describes one kind of multitasking: “It usually involves skimming the surface of the incoming data, picking out the relevant details, and moving on to the next stream. You’re paying attention, but only partially. That lets you cast a wider net, but it also runs the risk of keeping you from really studying the fish."[33]Multimedia pioneer Linda Stone coined the phrase "continuous partial attention" for this kind of processing.[34] Continuous partial attention is multitasking where things do not get studied in depth.
Rapidly increasing technology fosters multitasking because it promotes multiple sources of input at a given time. Instead of exchanging old equipment like TV, print, and music, for new equipment such as computers, the Internet, and video games, children and teens combine forms of media and continually increase sources of input.[35] According to studies by the Kaiser Family Foundation, in 1999 only 16 percent of time spent using media such as Internet, television, video games, telephones, text-messaging, or e-mail was combined. In 2005, 26 percent of the time these media were used together.[15] This increase in simultaneous media usage decreases the amount of attention paid to each device. In 2005 it was found that 82 percent of American youth use the Internet by the seventh grade in school.[36] A 2005 survey by the Kaiser Family Foundation found that, while their usage of media continued at a constant 6.5 hours per day, Americans ages 8 to 18 were crowding roughly 8.5 hours’ worth of media into their days due to multitasking. The survey showed that one quarter to one-third of the participants have more than one input “most of the time” while watching television, listening to music, or reading.[2] The 2007 Harvard Business Review featured Linda Stone's idea of “continuous partial attention,” or, “constantly scanning for opportunities and staying on top of contacts, events, and activities in an effort to miss nothing”.[15] As technology provides more distractions, attention is spread among tasks more thinly.
A prevalent example of this inattention to detail due to multitasking is apparent when people talk on cell phones while driving. One study found that having an accident is four times more likely when using a cell phone while driving.[37] Another study compared reaction times for experienced drivers during a number of tasks, and found that the subjects reacted more slowly to brake lights and stop signs during phone conversations than during other simultaneous tasks.[37] A 2006 study showed that drivers talking on cell phones were more involved in rear-end collisions and sped up slower than intoxicated drivers.[38] When talking, people must withdraw their attention from the road in order to formulate responses. Because the brain cannot focus on two sources of input at one time, driving and listening or talking, constantly changing input provided by cell phones distracts the brain and increases the likelihood of accidents.[39]
Supertasker
In 2010, a scientific study found that a small percent of the population appeared to be much better at multitasking than others, and these people were subsequently labeled "supertaskers".[40] In 2015, another study supported the idea of supertaskers. This particular study showed that they tested people by making them drive on a driving simulator while at the same time memorizing words and solving math problems. As expected, most of the participants did much worse than their individual task test scores. The supertaskers, however, were able to multitask without major effects to their performance.
Popular commentary on practical multitasking
Barry Schwartz has noted that, given the media-rich landscape of the Internet era, it is tempting to get into a habit of dwelling in a constant sea of information with too many choices, which has been noted to have a negative effect on human happiness.[41]
Observers of youth in modern society often comment upon the apparently advanced multitasking capabilities of the youngest generations of humans (Generation Y and Generation Z). While it is true that contemporary researchers find that youths in today's world exhibit high levels of multitasking, most experts believe that members of the Net Generation are not any better at multitasking than members of older generations.[42] However, some studies from the 2010's argue that Generation Y is becoming better at media multitasking. Media multitasking is when media consumers view several media platforms at the same time; such as watching TV while browsing the internet.[43] This is evidenced by the fact that they are gaining control over deciding which messages they pay attention to or not.[44] Nonetheless, while there is a great deal of evidence showing the negative effects of multitasking on cognitive tasks,[45][46][47][48] there is no evidence showing that multitasking has a positive or neutral effect on these tasks.
Many studies,[49][50][51][52][53] literature,[54] articles,[55][56][57] and worldwide consulting firms,[58] stress the fact that multitasking of any kind reduces the productivity and/or increases rate of errors, thus generating unnecessary frustrations.
In 2008, it was estimated that $650 billion[59] a year is wasted in US businesses due to multitasking.
^Heid, Markham (January 23, 2024). "That Smartphone in Your Hand Changes How You Walk". The New York Times. When scientists want to study stress, they often ask people to perform several tasks at once. That's because multitasking is a reliable way to stress people out.
^Hallowell, Edward M.. Crazy Busy: Overstretched, Overbooked, and About to Snap! Strategies for Handling Your Fast-Paced Life. 2007. Ballantine Books. ISBN0-345-48244-1
^Goldstein, E. Bruce (2015). Cognitive Psychology: Connecting Mind, Research, and Everyday Experience. Cengage Learning. pp. 100–102. ISBN978-1-285-76388-0.
^Klingberg, Torkel (2009). The Overflowing Brain: Information Overload and the Limits of Working Memory. Oxford: Oxford UP. pp. 7, 8. ISBN978-0-19-537288-5.
^Mealey, Linda (August 1994). "The Adapted Mind: Evolutionary Psychology and the Generation of Culture. Jerome Barkow, Leda Cosmides and John Tooby (eds.). New York: Oxford University Press, 1992. US$55.00. ISBN 0-19-50623-7. Oxford University Press, 200 Madison Ave., New York, NY 10021, USA". Politics and the Life Sciences. 13 (2): 294–295. doi:10.1017/s0730938400018700. ISSN0730-9384. S2CID151962878.
^Foehr, Ulla (December 2006). "MEDIA MULTITASKING AMONG AMERICAN YOUTH"(PDF). The Henry J. Kaiser Family Foundation. Kaiser Family Foundation. Retrieved August 3, 2010.
^ abDaniel L. Schacter; Daniel T. Gilbert; Daniel M. Wegner (2011) [2009]. Psychology (2nd ed.). Worth Publishers. pp. 132. ISBN978-1-4292-3719-2.
^Strayer, David L.; Drews, Frank A.; Crouch, Dennis J. (Summer 2006). "A Comparison of the Cell Phone Driver and the Drunk Driver". Human Factors: The Journal of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society. 48 (2): 381–391. doi:10.1518/001872006777724471. PMID16884056. S2CID7863538.(subscription required)
^Carrier, L Mark, Cheever, Nancy A, Rosen, Larry D, Benitez, Sandra, & Chang, Jennifer (2009). "Multitasking across generations: Multitasking choices and difficulty ratings in three generations of Americans", Computers in Human Behavior, Volume 25, p483–489.
^Koch I.; Lawo V.; Fels J.; Vorländer M. (2011). "Switching in the cocktail party: exploring intentional control of auditory selective attention". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 37 (4): 1140–1147. doi:10.1037/a0022189. PMID21553997.
^Strayer D. L.; Drews F. A. (2004). "Profiles in driver distraction: effects of cell phone conversations on younger and older drivers". Human Factors. 46 (4): 640–649. doi:10.1518/hfes.46.4.640.56806. PMID15709326. S2CID9218594.
^Wood, N., & Cowan, N. (1995). The cocktail party phenomenon revisited. How frequent are attention shifts to one’s name in an irrelevant auditory channel. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21(1), 255–260.
^Robert Rogers; Stephen Monsell (1995). "The costs of a predictable switch between simple cognitive tasks". Journal of Experimental Psychology. pp. 124, 207–231.
^Rubinstein, Joshua S.; Meyer, David E.; Evans, Jeffrey E. (2001). Executive Control of Cognitive Processes in Task Switching. Human Perception and Performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology.
^Crenshaw, Dave (2008). The myth of multitasking : how doing it all gets nothing done (1st ed.). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. p. 144. ISBN978-0-470-37225-8.
Appelbaum, Steven H.; Marchionni, Adam; Fernandez, Arturo (2008). "The multi-tasking paradox: perceptions, problems and strategies". Management Decision. 46 (9): 1313–1325. doi:10.1108/00251740810911966. S2CID17882403.
Gladstones, W. H.; Regan, M. A.; Lee, R. B. (1989). "Division of attention: The single-channel hypothesis revisited". Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology. 41 (A): 1–17. doi:10.1080/14640748908402350. S2CID146123937.
Ren, Dongning; Zhou, Haotian; Fu, Xiaolan (August 2009). "A Deeper Look at Gender Difference in Multitasking: Gender-Specific Mechanism of Cognitive Control". 2009 Fifth International Conference on Natural Computation. IEEE Xplore - digital library. pp. 13–17. doi:10.1109/ICNC.2009.542. ISBN978-0-7695-3736-8. S2CID1181140.
رئيس وزراء العراق قائمة رؤساء وزراء العراقشعار العراق شاغل المنصب محمد شياع السوداني منذ 28 أكتوبر 2022 عن المنصب المعين رئيس جمهورية العراق مدة الولاية 4 سنوات تأسيس المنصب 11 تشرين الأول 1920 أول حامل للمنصب عبد الرحمن الكيلاني النقيب[1] الموقع الرسمي http://www.pmo.iq/ تعديل مصدر...
Le informazioni riportate non sono consigli medici e potrebbero non essere accurate. I contenuti hanno solo fine illustrativo e non sostituiscono il parere medico: leggi le avvertenze. Clindamicina Caratteristiche generaliFormula bruta o molecolareC18H33ClN2O5S Massa molecolare (u)424,98 g/mol Numero CAS18323-44-9 Numero EINECS242-209-1 Codice ATCJ01FF01 PubChem29029 e 446598 DrugBankDB01190 SMILESCCCC1CC(N(C1)C)C(=O)NC(C2C(C(C(C(O2)SC)O)O)O)C(C)Cl Dati farmacocineticiBiodisponibilità90% Emi...
(The) Wild Wild West kan verwijzen naar: In film en televisie: The Wild Wild West (televisieserie), een televisieserie die liep van 1965 tot 1969 Wild Wild West (film), een film uit 1999 gebaseerd op deze serie. The Wild Wild West (film), een film uit 1921. In muziek: Wild Wild West (Kool Moe Dee), een single uit 1988 van Koel Moe Dee Wild Wild West (Will Smith), een single uit 1999 Wild, Wild West (Escape Club), een single uit 1988 van de Escape Club Wild Wild West (album), een album uit 198...
هذه المقالة يتيمة إذ تصل إليها مقالات أخرى قليلة جدًا. فضلًا، ساعد بإضافة وصلة إليها في مقالات متعلقة بها. (يوليو 2019) C27H31O16 هي صيغة كيميائية[1] تحتوي على 27 ذرة من الكربون و31 ذرة من الهيدروجين و16 ذرة من الأكسجين وتبلغ كتلتها المولية 611.52544 غ.مول-1. ومن المركبات الكيميائية التي
International incident between Venezuela and Colombia This article uses bare URLs, which are uninformative and vulnerable to link rot. Please consider converting them to full citations to ensure the article remains verifiable and maintains a consistent citation style. Several templates and tools are available to assist in formatting, such as reFill (documentation) and Citation bot (documentation). (August 2022) (Learn how and when to remove this template message) This article needs additional...
Iván Pérez Datos personalesNombre completo Iván Pérez PonsNacimiento La Habana5 de diciembre de 1971Nacionalidad(es) España EspañaAltura 1,97 metrosPeso 110 kilosCarrera deportivaDeporte WaterpoloClub profesionalDebut deportivo 1980Retirada deportiva 2012Selección España Medallero Campeonato Mundial de Waterpolo PlataRoma 2009waterpolo BronceMelbourne 2007waterpolo OroFukuoka 2001waterpolo Or...
Tel Dan adalah sebuah situs arkeologi di Israel yang terletak dekat dengan Dataran Tinggi Golan. Situs ini diidentifikasi sebagai kota kuno Dan yang dulunya bernama Laish. Dengan lokasinya yang dekat kepada perbatasan Lebanon, Tel Dan sering menjadi arena perang, terutama saat Perang Enam Hari tahun 1967. Identifikasi Tel Dan - Ekskavasi dan rekonstruksi gerbang timur dari Zaman Perunggu Tel Dan pertama kalinya diidentifikasi oleh Edward Robinson pada tahun 1838, dan telah dipastikan sebagai ...
German multinational store chain Not to be confused with Media market. This article contains content that is written like an advertisement. Please help improve it by removing promotional content and inappropriate external links, and by adding encyclopedic content written from a neutral point of view. (October 2023) (Learn how and when to remove this template message) Media-Saturn Holding GmbHMedia Markt store in Herstal, BelgiumTypeSubsidiaryIndustryRetailFounded1979FounderLeopold StiefelWalt...
20th-century Argentine Catholic priest This article's tone or style may not reflect the encyclopedic tone used on Wikipedia. See Wikipedia's guide to writing better articles for suggestions. (July 2018) (Learn how and when to remove this template message) Lorenzo Bartolomé MassaLorenzo Massa, c. 1910.Personal detailsBorn11 November 1882Morón Partido, Buenos AiresDied31 October 1949Provincia de Buenos AiresNationality ARGDenominationRoman Catholic ChurchOccupationCatholic priestEducatio...
This article needs to be updated. Please help update this article to reflect recent events or newly available information. (July 2017) Part of a series on theUN Security Councilresolutions Permanent members China France RussiaUnited Kingdom United States Non-permanent members Lists of resolutions Resolutions 1 to 1000 (1946–1995) 001 to 0100 (1946–1953) 101 to 0200 (1953–1965) 201 to 0300 (1965–1971) 301 to 0400 (1971–1976)...
Football clubBratstvo GračanicaFull nameNogometni klub Bratstvo GračanicaFounded1945; 78 years ago (1945)GroundStadion Luke, GračanicaCapacity5,200ChairmanNusret HelićManagerNikola NikićLeagueFirst League of FBiH2022–23First League of FBiH, 12th Home colours Away colours Nogometni klub Bratstvo Gračanica or simply NK Bratstvo Gračanica is a professional association football club from the city of Gračanica that is situated in Bosnia and Herzegovina. Bratstvo current...
This article is about the college in Liverpool. For other schools and colleges of the same name, see St Francis Xavier College (disambiguation). 11–18 boys academy in Liverpool, Merseyside, EnglandSt. Francis Xavier's College School crest St Francis Xavier's (SFX) College buildingAddressWoolton Hill Road WooltonLiverpool, Merseyside, L25 6EGEnglandCoordinates53°22′58″N 2°52′49″W / 53.382662°N 2.880281°W / 53.382662; -2.880281InformationType11–18 boys Ac...
Hacker group This article needs to be updated. Please help update this article to reflect recent events or newly available information. (October 2021) RedHack is a Turkish Marxist-Leninist computer hacker group founded in 1997.[1] The group has claimed responsibility for hacking the websites of institutions which include the Council of Higher Education, Turkish police forces, the Turkish Army, Türk Telekom, and the National Intelligence Organization others.[2] The group's cor...
Mountjoy Prison where on October 31, 1973, three IRA members escaped in a hijacked helicopter[1] Prison de la Santé where on May 26, 1986, Michel Vaujour was flown out of the prison by his wife[2] There have been multiple prison escapes where an inmate escapes by means of a helicopter. One of the earliest instances was the escape of Joel David Kaplan, nicknamed Man Fan, on August 19, 1971, from the Santa Martha Acatitla in Mexico.[3] Kaplan was a New York businessman ...
River in Alberta, Canada Ghost RiverMount Aylmer with the North Ghost in the foregroundLocationCountryCanadaProvinceAlbertaPhysical characteristicsSourceSouth slopes of Mount Oliver • coordinates51°24′38″N 115°29′20″W / 51.41056°N 115.48889°W / 51.41056; -115.48889 MouthBow River at Ghost Lake • coordinates51°13′26″N 114°42′48″W / 51.22389°N 114.71333°W / 51.22389; -114.71333Basin size...
American singer based in South Korea (born 1991) For other people named David Oh, see David Oh (disambiguation). David OhBackground informationAlso known asOh Se-hunBorn (1991-06-29) June 29, 1991 (age 32)San Francisco, CaliforniaGenresK-popOccupation(s)SingerYears active2010-presentLabelslocalhigh records(JMG)Korean nameHangul오세훈Revised RomanizationO Se-hunMcCune–ReischauerO Sehun Musical artist David Oh (Korean: 데이비드 오, Korean name: Oh Se-hun; Hangul: 오세...
Bruce AbbottBruce Abbott foto publisitas untuk Summer Heat (1988)LahirBruce Paul Abbott28 Juli 1954 (umur 69)Portland, Oregon, A.S.PekerjaanAktorTahun aktif1982–sekarangSuami/istriKathleen Quinlan (1994–sekarang)Linda Hamilton (1982–1989) Bruce Paul Abbott (lahir 28 Juli 1954) adalah seorang aktor asal Amerika Serikat. Ia bermain dalam film seperti Re-Animator, Bad Dreams, The Prophecy II, Out of Time, dan Bride of Re-Animator, dan serial TV Dark Justice. Karier Abbott mulai ...
2007 live album by Katy Garbi18 Hronia Live18 Χρόνια LiveLive album by Katy GarbiReleasedDecember 5, 2007GenrePop, Modern Laika, DanceLabelSony BMG Greece/ColumbiaProducerPossidonas GiannopoulosKaty Garbi chronology Pos Allazei O Kairos(2006) 18 Hronia Live18 Χρόνια Live(2007) Kainouria Ego(2008) Singles from 18 Hronia Live Mi Ta Filas Ta Matia Mou 18 Hronia Live Medley Se Sfalma Epeses Kardia Mou I Proti Mas Fora 18 Hronia Live (Greek: 18 Χρόνια Live; English: 18 Ye...
У Вікіпедії є статті про інші значення цього терміна: Чорний тюльпан (значення). «Евакуація 200» — гуманітарний проєкт Управління цивільно-військового співробітництва Збройних сил України, завданням якого є пошук та евакуація тіл військовослужбовців ЗСУ, представник...
Strategi Solo vs Squad di Free Fire: Cara Menang Mudah!