Confabulation is a memory error consisting of the production of fabricated, distorted, or misinterpreted memories about oneself or the world. It is generally associated with certain types of brain damage (especially aneurysm in the anterior communicating artery) or a specific subset of dementias.[1] While still an area of ongoing research, the basal forebrain is implicated in the phenomenon of confabulation. People who confabulate present with incorrect memories ranging from subtle inaccuracies to surreal fabrications, and may include confusion or distortion in the temporal framing (timing, sequence or duration) of memories.[2] In general, they are very confident about their recollections, even when challenged with contradictory evidence.[3]
Confabulation occurs when individuals mistakenly recall false information, without intending to deceive. Brain damage, dementia, and anticholinergic toxidrome can cause this distortion. Two types of confabulation exist: provoked and spontaneous, with two distinctions: verbal and behavioral. Verbal statements, false information, and the patient's unawareness of the distortion are all associated with this phenomenon. Personality structure also plays a role in confabulation.
Numerous theories have been developed to explain confabulation. Neuropsychological theories suggest that cognitive dysfunction causes the distortion. Self-identity theories posit that people confabulate to preserve themselves. The temporality theory believes that confabulation occurs when an individual cannot place events properly in time. The monitoring and strategic retrieval account theories argue that confabulation arises when individuals cannot recall memories correctly or monitor them after retrieval. The executive control and fuzzy-trace theories also attempt to explain why confabulation happens.
Confabulation can occur with nervous system injuries or illnesses, including Korsakoff's syndrome, Alzheimer's disease, schizophrenia, and traumatic brain injury. It is believed that the right frontal lobe of the brain is damaged, causing false memories. Children are especially susceptible to forced confabulation as they are highly impressionable. Feedback can increase confidence in false memories. In rare cases, confabulation occurs in ordinary individuals.
Different memory tests, including recognition tasks and free recall tasks, can be used to study confabulation. Treatment depends on the underlying cause of the distortion. Ongoing research aims to develop a standard test battery to discern between different types of confabulations, distinguish delusions from confabulations, understand the role of unconscious processes, and identify pathological and nonpathological confabulations.
Description
Confabulation was originally defined as "the emergence of memories of events and experiences which never took place".[4][5]
Confabulation is distinguished from lying as there is no intent to deceive and the person is unaware the information is false.[6] Although individuals can present blatantly false information, confabulation can also seem to be coherent, internally consistent, and relatively normal.[6]
Confabulated memories of all types most often occur in autobiographical memory and are indicative of a complicated and intricate process that can be led astray at any point during encoding, storage, or recall of a memory.[3] This type of confabulation is commonly seen in Korsakoff's syndrome.[8]
Distinctions
Several forms of confabulation have been distinguished which vary in terms of behaviour, mechanism, and location of brain damage.[9] The most common distinction is between the following two types:
Provoked confabulations represent a normal response to a faulty memory, are common in both amnesia and dementia,[10] and can become apparent during memory tests.[11]
Spontaneous confabulations do not occur in response to a cue[11] and seem to be involuntary.[12] They are relatively rare, more common in cases of dementia, and may result from the interaction between frontal lobe pathology and organic amnesia.[10] A subgroup of patients at least occasionally act according to their confabulations betraying a confusion of current reality.[13][14] These patients are always disoriented about their current role, location, and time. This form has been called behaviorally spontaneous confabulation.[9][5] It is strongly associated with damage, disconnection, or dysfunction of the posterior medial orbitofrontal cortex (area 13)[15] and appears to have a specific mechanism: a failure of "orbitofrontal reality filtering", a preconscious mechanism normally verifying whether an upcoming thought refers to current reality or not.[16][5]
Signs and symptoms
Confabulation is associated with several characteristics:
False verbal statements that may include autobiographical and non-personal information, such as historical facts, fairy-tales, or other aspects of semantic memory.
The account is normally coherent and is usually drawn from the patient's memory of actual experiences. Very rarely, it is fantastic.
Both the premise and the details of the account can be false.
The patient is unaware of the accounts' distortions or inappropriateness, and is not concerned when errors are pointed out.
There is no hidden motivation behind the account.
The patient's personality structure may play a role in their readiness to confabulate.[6]
Theories
Theories of confabulation range in emphasis. Some theories propose that confabulations represent a way for memory disabled people to maintain their self-identity.[11] Other theories use neurocognitive links to explain the process of confabulation.[17] Still other theories frame confabulation around the more familiar concept of delusion.[18] Other researchers frame confabulation within the fuzzy-trace theory.[19] Finally, some researchers call for theories that rely less on neurocognitive explanations and more on epistemic accounts.[20]
Neuropsychological theories
The most popular theories of confabulation come from the field of neuropsychology or cognitive neuroscience.[17] Controlled experimental evidence is, however, scarce.[5] Research suggests that confabulation is associated with dysfunction of cognitive processes that control the retrieval from long-term memory. Frontal lobe damage often disrupts this process, preventing the retrieval of information and the evaluation of its output.[21][22] Furthermore, researchers argue that confabulation is a disorder resulting from failed "reality monitoring/source monitoring" (i.e. deciding whether a memory is based on an actual event or whether it is imagined).[23] Some neuropsychologists suggest that errors in retrieval of information from long-term memory that are made by normal subjects involve different components of control processes than errors made by confabulators.[24] Fantastic confabulations have been attributed to a dysfunction of the Supervisory System,[25] which is believed to be a function of the frontal cortex.
Temporality theory
Support for the temporality account suggests that confabulations occur when an individual is unable to place events properly in time.[11] Thus, an individual might correctly state an action they performed, but say they did it yesterday, when they did it weeks ago. In the Memory, Consciousness, and Temporality Theory, confabulation occurs because of a deficit in temporal consciousness or awareness.[26]
Monitoring theory
Along a similar notion are the theories of reality and source monitoring theories.[12] In these theories, confabulation occurs when individuals incorrectly attribute memories as reality, or incorrectly attribute memories to a certain source. Thus, an individual might claim an imagined event happened in reality, or that a friend told him/her about an event he/she actually heard about on television.
Strategic retrieval account theory
Supporters of the strategic retrieval account suggest that confabulations occur when an individual cannot actively monitor a memory for truthfulness after its retrieval.[12] An individual recalls a memory, but there is some deficit after recall that interferes with the person establishing its falseness.
Executive control theory
Still others propose that all types of false memories, including confabulation, fit into a general memory and executive function model.[27] In 2007, a framework for confabulation was proposed that stated confabulation is the result of two things: Problems with executive control and problems with evaluation. In the executive control deficit, the incorrect memory is retrieved from the brain. In the evaluative deficit, the memory will be accepted as a truth due to an inability to distinguish a belief from an actual memory.[11]
In the context of delusion theories
Recent models of confabulation have attempted to build upon the link between delusion and confabulation.[18] More recently, a monitoring account for delusion, applied to confabulation, proposed both the inclusion of conscious and unconscious processing. The claim was that by encompassing the notion of both processes, spontaneous versus provoked confabulations could be better explained. In other words, there are two ways to confabulate. One is the unconscious, spontaneous way in which a memory goes through no logical, explanatory processing. The other is the conscious, provoked way in which a memory is recalled intentionally by the individual to explain something confusing or unusual.[28]
Fuzzy-trace theory
Fuzzy-trace theory, or FTT, is a concept more commonly applied to the explanation of judgement decisions.[19] According to this theory, memories are encoded generally (gist), as well as specifically (verbatim). Thus, a confabulation could result from recalling the incorrect verbatim memory or from being able to recall the gist portion, but not the verbatim portion, of a memory.
FTT uses a set of five principles to explain false-memory phenomena. Principle 1 suggests that subjects store verbatim information and gist information parallel to one another. Both forms of storage involve the surface content of an experience. Principle 2 shares factors of retrieval of gist and verbatim traces. Principle 3 is based on dual-opponent processes in false memory. Generally, gist retrieval supports false memory, while verbatim retrieval suppresses it. Developmental variability is the topic of Principle 4. As a child develops into an adult, there is obvious improvement in the acquisition, retention, and retrieval of both verbatim and gist memory. However, during late adulthood, there will be a decline in these abilities. Finally, Principle 5 explains that verbatim and gist processing cause vivid remembering. Fuzzy-trace Theory, governed by these 5 principles, has proved useful in explaining false memory and generating new predictions about it.[29]
Epistemic theory
However, not all accounts are so embedded in the neurocognitive aspects of confabulation. Some attribute confabulation to epistemic accounts.[20] In 2009, theories underlying the causation and mechanisms for confabulation were criticized for their focus on neural processes, which are somewhat unclear, as well as their emphasis on the negativity of false remembering. Researchers proposed that an epistemic account of confabulation would be more encompassing of both the advantages and disadvantages of the process.
Presentation
Associated neurological and psychological conditions
Wernicke–Korsakoff syndrome is a neurological disorder typically characterized by years of alcohol use disorder characterized by excessive alcohol consumption and a nutritional thiamine deficiency.[30] Confabulation is one salient symptom of this syndrome.[31][32] A study on confabulation in Korsakoff's patients found that they are subject to provoked confabulation when prompted with questions pertaining to episodic memory, not semantic memory, and when prompted with questions where the appropriate response would be "I don't know."[33] This suggests that confabulation in these patients is "domain-specific." Korsakoff's patients who confabulate are more likely than healthy adults to falsely recognize distractor words, suggesting that false recognition is a "confabulatory behavior."
Alzheimer's disease is a condition with both neurological and psychological components. It is a form of dementia associated with severe frontal lobe dysfunction. Confabulation in individuals with Alzheimer's is often more spontaneous than it is in other conditions, especially in the advanced stages of the disease. Alzheimer's patients demonstrate comparable abilities to encode information as healthy elderly adults, suggesting that impairments in encoding are not associated with confabulation.[34] However, as seen in Korsakoff's patients, confabulation in Alzheimer's patients is higher when prompted with questions investigating episodic memory. Researchers suggest this is due to damage in the posterior cortical regions of the brain, which is a symptom characteristic of Alzheimer's disease.
Schizophrenia is a psychological disorder in which confabulation is sometimes observed. Although confabulation is usually coherent in its presentation, confabulations of schizophrenic patients are often delusional.[35] Researchers have noted that these patients tend to make up delusions on the spot which are often fantastic and become increasingly elaborate with questioning.[36] Unlike patients with Korsakoff's and Alzheimer's, patients with schizophrenia are more likely to confabulate when prompted with questions regarding their semantic memories, as opposed to episodic memory prompting.[37] In addition, confabulation does not appear to be related to any memory deficit in schizophrenic patients. This is contrary to most forms of confabulation. Also, confabulations made by schizophrenic patients often do not involve the creation of new information, but instead involve an attempt by the patient to reconstruct actual details of a past event.
Traumatic brain injury (TBI) can also result in confabulation. Research has shown that patients with damage to the inferior medial frontal lobe confabulate significantly more than patients with damage to the posterior area and healthy controls.[38] This suggests that this region is key in producing confabulatory responses, and that memory deficit is important but not necessary in confabulation. Additionally, research suggests that confabulation can be seen in patients with frontal lobe syndrome, which involves an insult to the frontal lobe as a result of disease or traumatic brain injury (TBI).[39][40][41] Finally, rupture of the anterior or posterior communicating artery, subarachnoid hemorrhage, and encephalitis are also possible causes of confabulation.[21][42]
Location of brain lesions
Confabulation is believed to be a result of damage to the right frontal lobe of the brain.[6] In particular, damage can be localized to the ventromedial frontal lobes and other structures fed by the anterior communicating artery (ACoA), including the basal forebrain, septum, fornix, cingulate gyrus, cingulum, anterior hypothalamus, and head of the caudate nucleus.[43][44]
While some recent literature has suggested that older adults may be more susceptible than their younger counterparts to have false memories, the majority of research on forced confabulation centers around children.[45] Children are particularly susceptible to forced confabulations based on their high suggestibility.[46][47] When forced to recall confabulated events, children are less likely to remember that they had previously confabulated these situations, and they are more likely than their adult counterparts to come to remember these confabulations as real events that transpired.[48] Research suggests that this inability to distinguish between past confabulatory and real events is centered on developmental differences in source monitoring. Due to underdeveloped encoding and critical reasoning skills, children's ability to distinguish real memories from false memories may be impaired. It may also be that younger children lack the meta-memory processes required to remember confabulated versus non-confabulated events.[49] Children's meta-memory processes may also be influenced by expectancies or biases, in that they believe that highly plausible false scenarios are not confabulated.[50] However, when knowingly being tested for accuracy, children are more likely to respond, "I don't know" at a rate comparable to adults for unanswerable questions than they are to confabulate.[51][52] Ultimately, misinformation effects can be minimized by tailoring individual interviews to the specific developmental stage, often based on age, of the participant.[53]
Provoked versus spontaneous confabulations
There is evidence to support different cognitive mechanisms for provoked and spontaneous confabulation.[54] One study suggested that spontaneous confabulation may be a result of an amnesic patient's inability to distinguish the chronological order of events in their memory. In contrast, provoked confabulation may be a compensatory mechanism, in which the patient tries to make up for their memory deficiency by attempting to demonstrate competency in recollection.
Confidence in false memories
Confabulation of events or situations may lead to an eventual acceptance of the confabulated information as true.[55] For instance, people who knowingly lie about a situation may eventually come to believe that their lies are truthful with time.[56] In an interview setting, people are more likely to confabulate in situations in which they are presented false information by another person, as opposed to when they self-generate these falsehoods.[57] Further, people are more likely to accept false information as true when they are interviewed at a later time (after the event in question) than those who are interviewed immediately or soon after the event.[58] Affirmative feedback for confabulated responses is also shown to increase the confabulator's confidence in their response.[59] For instance, in culprit identification, if a witness falsely identifies a member of a line-up, he will be more confident in his identification if the interviewer provides affirmative feedback. This effect of confirmatory feedback appears to last over time, as witnesses will even remember the confabulated information months later.[60]
Among normal subjects
On rare occasions, confabulation can also be seen in normal subjects.[24] It is currently unclear how completely healthy individuals produce confabulations. It is possible that these individuals are in the process of developing some type of organic condition that is causing their confabulation symptoms. It is not uncommon, however, for the general population to display some very mild symptoms of provoked confabulations. Subtle distortions and intrusions in memory are commonly produced by normal subjects when they remember something poorly.
Diagnosis and treatment
Spontaneous confabulations, due to their involuntary nature, cannot be manipulated in a laboratory setting.[12] However, provoked confabulations can be researched in various theoretical contexts. The mechanisms found to underlie provoked confabulations can be applied to spontaneous confabulation mechanisms. The basic premise of researching confabulation comprises finding errors and distortions in memory tests of an individual.
Deese–Roediger–McDermott lists
Confabulations can be detected in the context of the Deese–Roediger–McDermott paradigm by using the Deese–Roediger–McDermott lists.[61] Participants listen to audio recordings of several lists of words centered around a theme, known as the critical word. The participants are later asked to recall the words on their list. If the participant recalls the critical word, which was never explicitly stated in the list, it is considered a confabulation. Participants often have a false memory for the critical word.
Recognition tasks
Confabulations can also be researched by using continuous recognition tasks.[12] These tasks are often used in conjunction with confidence ratings. Generally, in a recognition task, participants are rapidly presented with pictures. Some of these pictures are shown once; others are shown multiple times. Participants press a key if they have seen the picture previously. Following a period of time, participants repeat the task. More errors on the second task, versus the first, are indicative of confusion, representing false memories.
Free recall tasks
Confabulations can also be detected using a free recall task, such as a self-narrative task.[12] Participants are asked to recall stories (semantic or autobiographical) that are highly familiar to them. The stories recalled are encoded for errors that could be classified as distortions in memory. Distortions could include falsifying true story elements or including details from a completely different story. Errors such as these would be indicative of confabulations.
Treatment
Treatment for confabulation is somewhat dependent on the cause or source, if identifiable. For example, treatment of Wernicke–Korsakoff syndrome involves large doses of vitamin B in order to reverse the thiamine deficiency.[62] If there is no known physiological cause, more general cognitive techniques may be used to treat confabulation. A case study published in 2000 showed that Self-Monitoring Training (SMT)[63] reduced delusional confabulations. Furthermore, improvements were maintained at a three-month follow-up and were found to generalize to everyday settings. Although this treatment seems promising, more rigorous research is necessary to determine the efficacy of SMT in the general confabulation population.
Research
Although significant gains have been made in the understanding of confabulation in recent years, there is still much to be learned. One group of researchers in particular has laid out several important questions for future study. They suggest more information is needed regarding the neural systems that support the different cognitive processes necessary for normal source monitoring. They also proposed the idea of developing a standard neuropsychological test battery able to discriminate between the different types of confabulations. And there is a considerable amount of debate regarding the best approach to organizing and combining neuro-imaging, pharmacological, and cognitive/behavioral approaches to understand confabulation.[64]
In a recent review article, another group of researchers contemplate issues concerning the distinctions between delusions and confabulation. They question whether delusions and confabulation should be considered distinct or overlapping disorders and, if overlapping, to what degree? They also discuss the role of unconscious processes in confabulation. Some researchers suggest that unconscious emotional and motivational processes are potentially just as important as cognitive and memory problems. Finally, they raise the question of where to draw the line between the pathological and the nonpathological. Delusion-like beliefs and confabulation-like fabrications are commonly seen in healthy individuals. What are the important differences between patients with similar etiology who do and do not confabulate? Since the line between pathological and nonpathological is likely blurry, should we take a more dimensional approach to confabulation? Research suggests that confabulation occurs along a continuum of implausibility, bizarreness, content, conviction, preoccupation, and distress, and impact on daily life.[65]
^Berrios G E (1998) Confabulations: A Conceptual History. Journal of the History of the Neurosciences. 7: 225-241.
^Pendick, Daniel (Summer 2000). "The truth about confabulation". Memory Loss & the Brain: Newsletter of the Memory Disorders Project. Rutgers University-Newark. Archived from the original on 3 January 2013. Retrieved 7 April 2022.
^ abMatthews, Paul M.; McClelland, James L. (2010). Nalbantian, Suzanne (ed.). The memory process: neuroscientific and humanistic perspectives. Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press. ISBN978-0-262-01457-1.
^Wernicke, Carl (1900). Grundriss der Psychiatrie in klinischen Vorlesungen. Leipzig: Thieme. p. 139.
^ abcdMoscovitch M. 1995. "Confabulation". In (Eds. Schacter D.L., Coyle J.T., Fischbach G.D., Mesulum M.M. & Sullivan L.G.), Memory Distortion. pp. 226–51. Cambridge, Massachusetts: Harvard University Press.
^Sivolap IuP Damulin IV (2013). "[Wernicke encephalopathy and Korsakoff's psychosis: clinical-pathophysiological correlation, diagnostics and treatment]". Zh Nevrol Psikhiatr Im S S Korsakova. 113 (6 Pt 2): 20–26. PMID23887463.
^ abcdeMetcalf, Kasey; Langdon, Robyn; Coltheart, Max (1 February 2007). "Models of confabulation: A critical review and a new framework". Cognitive Neuropsychology. 24 (1): 23–47. doi:10.1080/02643290600694901. PMID18416482. S2CID45885109.
^Schnider, A; von Däniken, C; Gutbrod, K (1996). "The mechanisms of spontaneous and provoked confabulations". Brain : A Journal of Neurology. 119 ( Pt 4) (4): 1365–75. doi:10.1093/brain/119.4.1365. PMID8813298.
^Schnider, A; Ptak, R (1999). "Spontaneous confabulators fail to suppress currently irrelevant memory traces". Nature Neuroscience. 2 (7): 677–81. doi:10.1038/10236. PMID10404203.
^ abSchnider, A (2003). "Spontaneous confabulation and the adaptation of thought to ongoing reality". Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 4 (8): 662–71. doi:10.1038/nrn1179. PMID12894241.
^ abGlowinski, Remy; Payman, Vahid; Frencham, Kate (2008). "Confabulation: a spontaneous and fantastic review". Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry. 42 (11): 932–40. doi:10.1080/00048670802415335. PMID18941957. S2CID23586700.
^ abBrainerd, C.J.; Reyna, V.F. (1 November 1998). "Fuzzy-Trace Theory and Children's False Memories". Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 71 (2): 81–129. doi:10.1006/jecp.1998.2464. PMID9843617. S2CID12290995.
^ abBortolotti, Lisa; Cox, Rochelle E. (1 December 2009). "'Faultless' ignorance: Strengths and limitations of epistemic definitions of confabulation". Consciousness and Cognition. 18 (4): 952–65. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2009.08.011. PMID19773185. S2CID35121072.
^ abBaddeley, Alan; Wilson, Barbara (1986). "Amnesia, autobiographical memory, and confabulation". In Rubin, David C. (ed.). Autobiographical Memory. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. pp. 225–52. doi:10.1017/CBO9780511558313.020. ISBN9780511558313.
^Johnson, M.K. (1991). Reality monitoring: Evidence from confabulation in organic brain disease patients. In G.P. Prigatano & D.L. Schacter (Eds.), Awareness of deficit after brain injury. pp. 176–97. New York: Oxford.
^Norman, D.A., & Shallice, T. (1980). Attention to action. Willed and automatic control of behavior. University of California San Diego CHIP Report 99. Later published as: Norman, Donald A.; Shallice, Tim (1986). "Attention to Action: Willed and Automatic Control of Behavior". Consciousness and Self-Regulation. pp. 1–18. doi:10.1007/978-1-4757-0629-1_1. ISBN978-1-4757-0631-4.
^Dalla Barba, Gianfranco; Boissé, Marie-Françoise (2010). "Temporal consciousness and confabulation: Is the medial temporal lobe "temporal"?". Cognitive Neuropsychiatry. 15 (1–3): 95–117. doi:10.1080/13546800902758017. PMID19750399. S2CID42447646.
^Kopelman, Michael D. (1 May 1999). "Varieties of False Memory". Cognitive Neuropsychology. 16 (3–5): 197–214. doi:10.1080/026432999380762.
^Brainerd, C. J., & Reyna, V. F. (2002). Fuzzy-Trace Theory and False Memory. Current Directions In Psychological Science (Wiley–Blackwell), 11(5), 164–69.
^Homewood, J; Bond, N. W. (1999). "Thiamin deficiency and Korsakoff's syndrome: Failure to find memory impairments following nonalcoholic Wernicke's encephalopathy". Alcohol. 19 (1): 75–84. doi:10.1016/s0741-8329(99)00027-0. PMID10487391.
^Wing, J. K., Cooper, J. E., Sartorius, N. 1974. The description and classification of psychiatric symptoms: An instruction manual for the PSE and catego system. London: Cambridge University Press.
^Lorente-Rovira, E; Pomarol-Clotet, E; McCarthy, R. A.; Berrios, G. E.; McKenna, P. J. (2007). "Confabulation in schizophrenia and its relationship to clinical and neuropsychological features of the disorder". Psychological Medicine. 37 (10): 1403–12. doi:10.1017/S0033291707000566. PMID17506924. S2CID32121170.
^Lorente-Rovira, E; Santos-Gómez, J. L.; Moro, M; Villagrán, J. M.; McKenna, P. J. (1 November 2010). "Confabulation in schizophrenia: A neuropsychological study". Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 16 (6): 1018–26. doi:10.1017/S1355617710000718. hdl:10234/88477. PMID20630120. S2CID23194952.
^Baddeley, Alan (1996). "Exploring the Central Executive". Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 49A (1): 5–28. doi:10.1080/713755608. S2CID54633639.
^Dalla Barba G (1993). "Confabulation: knowledge and recollective experience". Cognitive Neuropsychology. 10 (1): 1–20. doi:10.1080/02643299308253454.
^Irle, E; Wowra, B; Kunert, H. J.; Hampl, J; Kunze, S (1992). "Memory disturbances following anterior communicating artery rupture". Annals of Neurology. 31 (5): 473–80. doi:10.1002/ana.410310503. PMID1596082. S2CID42233017.
^Brainerd, C. J.; Reyna, V. F.; Ceci, S. J. (2008). "Developmental reversals in false memory: A review of data and theory". Psychological Bulletin. 134 (3): 343–82. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.134.3.343. PMID18444700.
^Shapiro, Lauren R.; Purdy, Telisa L. (2005). "Suggestibility and source monitoring errors: blame the interview style, interviewer consistency, and the child's personality". Applied Cognitive Psychology. 19 (4): 489–506. doi:10.1002/acp.1093.
^Shapiro, Lauren R.; Blackford, Cheryl; Chen, Chiung-Fen (2005). "Eyewitness memory for a simulated misdemeanor crime: the role of age and temperament in suggestibility". Applied Cognitive Psychology. 19 (3): 267–89. doi:10.1002/acp.1089.
^Ackil, Jennifer K.; Zaragoza, Maria S. (1 November 1998). "Memorial consequences of forced confabulation: Age differences in susceptibility to false memories". Developmental Psychology. 34 (6): 1358–72. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.34.6.1358. PMID9823517.
^Ghetti, Simona; Castelli, Paola; Lyons, Kristen E. (2010). "Knowing about not remembering: developmental dissociations in lack-of-memory monitoring". Developmental Science. 13 (4): 611–21. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00908.x. PMID20590725.
^Ghetti, Simona; Alexander, Kristen Weede (2004). ""If It Happened, I Would Remember It": Strategic Use of Event Memorability in the Rejection of False Autobiographical Events". Child Development. 75 (2): 542–61. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00692.x. PMID15056205.
^Roebers, Claudia; Fernandez, Olivia (2002). "The Effects of Accuracy Motivation on Children's and Adults' Event Recall, Suggestibility, and Their Answers to Unanswerable Questions". Journal of Cognition and Development. 3 (4): 415–43. doi:10.1207/S15327647JCD3,4-03.
^Scoboria, Alan; Mazzoni, Giuliana; Kirsch, Irving (2008). ""Don't know" responding to answerable and unanswerable questions during misleading and hypnotic interviews". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied. 14 (3): 255–65. doi:10.1037/1076-898X.14.3.255. PMID18808279.
^Holliday, Robyn E.; Albon, Amanda J. (2004). "Minimising misinformation effects in young children with cognitive interview mnemonics". Applied Cognitive Psychology. 18 (3): 263–81. doi:10.1002/acp.973.
^Polage, Danielle C. (2004). "Fabrication deflation? The mixed effects of lying on memory". Applied Cognitive Psychology. 18 (4): 455–65. doi:10.1002/acp.995.
^Pezdek, Kathy; Lam, Shirley T.; Sperry, Kathryn (2009). "Forced confabulation more strongly influences event memory if suggestions are other-generated than self-generated". Legal and Criminological Psychology. 14 (2): 241–52. doi:10.1348/135532508X344773.
^Melnyk, Laura; Bruck, Maggie (2004). "Timing moderates the effects of repeated suggestive interviewing on children's eyewitness memory". Applied Cognitive Psychology. 18 (5): 613–31. doi:10.1002/acp.1013.
^Hafstad, Gertrud Sofie; Memon, Amina; Logie, Robert (2004). "Post-identification feedback, confidence and recollections of witnessing conditions in child witnesses". Applied Cognitive Psychology. 18 (7): 901–12. doi:10.1002/acp.1037.
^Zaragoza, M. S.; Payment, K. E.; Ackil, J. K.; Drivdahl, S. B.; Beck, M. (2001). "Interviewing Witnesses: Forced Confabulation and Confirmatory Feedback Increase False Memories". Psychological Science. 12 (6): 473–77. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00388. PMID11760134. S2CID11951759.
O eclipse solar de 25 de outubro de 2022 foi um eclipse solar parcial visível na Europa, na Sibéria, no Oriente Médio, na Ásia Ocidental e no nordeste da África. A fase máxima do eclipse parcial foi registrada na Sibéria perto de Nizhnevartovsk na Rússia. É o eclipse número 55 na série Saros 124 e teve magnitude 0.8623.[1] Referências ↑ Espenak, Fred. «Partial Solar Eclipse of 2022 Oct 25». NASA Eclipse Website. Consultado em 27 de maio de 2021 Precedido por30 de a...
Magic Hour[1]Sutradara Asep Kusdinar Produser Sukhdev Singh Wicky V. Olindo Ditulis oleh Tisa TS BerdasarkanMagic Houroleh Tisa TS & Stanley Meulen)PemeranDimas AnggaraMichelle ZiudithRizky NazarNadya ArinaMeriam BellinaSurya SaputraIra WibowoAnisa RahmaMaeeva AminAdinda ThomasPerusahaanproduksiScreenplay FilmsTanggal rilis13 Agustus 2015 (2015-08-13)Durasi96 menitNegaraIndonesiaBahasa Indonesia Magic Hour, adalah film drama komedi Indonesia yang dirilis pada tanggal 13 Agust...
Brand of ketchup and brown sauce This article is about the brand name. For the plural of daddy, see Father. For the 1924 silent film, see Daddies (film). DaddiesProduct typeSauceOwnerKraft Heinz (2005–pres.)Produced byHeinzCountryUnited KingdomIntroduced1904; 119 years ago (1904) Birmingham, EnglandMarketsEuropePrevious ownersHP Foods Daddies is a brand of ketchup and brown sauce in the United Kingdom.[1][2] History The brown sauce product, known as Da...
Duke of Vendôme Prince EmmanuelDuke of VendômeBorn(1872-01-18)18 January 1872Meran, Austria-HungaryDied1 February 1931(1931-02-01) (aged 59)Cannes, French Third RepublicBurialChapelle royale de DreuxSpouse Princess Henriette of Belgium (m. 1896)IssueMarie Louise, Princess Philip of Bourbon Two-SiciliesPrincess SophiePrincess GenevièvePrince Charles Philippe, Duke of NemoursHouseOrléansFatherPrince Ferdinand, Duke of AlençonMotherDuchess Sophie ...
This article needs additional citations for verification. Please help improve this article by adding citations to reliable sources. Unsourced material may be challenged and removed.Find sources: The Japanese Sandman – news · newspapers · books · scholar · JSTOR (August 2021) (Learn how and when to remove this template message) 1920 song by Paul Whiteman and His Ambassador OrchestraThe Japanese SandmanSong by Paul Whiteman and His Ambassador OrchestraB-...
Kamus Indoneenglish (bahasa Inggris: An English-Indonesian Dictionary) adalah kamus dwibahasa yang diterbitkan aslinya oleh Cornell University Press dan kemudian diterbitkan di Indonesia oleh Gramedia Pustaka Utama yang disusun oleh ahli bahasa dari Amerika Serikat yang bernama John M. Echols, dan ahli bahasa dari Indonesia yang bernama Hassan Shadily. Kamus ini terdapat puluhan ribu lema, tulisan fonetik yang mendampingi setiap lema untuk mengetahui cara pelafalan, penjelasan kelas kata,...
Selecciones clasificadas para el Mundial de 2022 Selecciones que no se clasificaron Equipos suspendidos Países que no son miembros de la FIFA La XXII Copa Mundial de Fútbol se celebró en Catar entre el 20 de noviembre y el 18 de diciembre de 2022. Para su fase final se clasificaron 32 selecciones. Dichas selecciones fueron divididas en 8 grupos de cuatro, para posteriorme...
Satirical play by Niccolò Machiavelli For other uses, see Mandrake (disambiguation). The MandrakeCover plate from 1556 editionWritten byNiccolò MachiavelliCharacters Callimaco Siro Messer Nicia Ligurio Sostrata Friar Timoteo A woman Lucrezia Date premieredCarnival season, 1526 (1526)Place premieredFlorence, ItalyOriginal languageItalianGenreComedySettingThe Florentine Republic, 1504 The Mandrake (Italian: La Mandragola [la manˈdraːɡola]) is a satirical play by Italian Renais...
American racing driver Buddy RiceBuddy Rice waits for a qualification attempt before the 2007 Indianapolis 500.Nationality AmericanBorn (1976-01-31) January 31, 1976 (age 47)Phoenix, Arizona, U.S.IndyCar Series careerDebut season2002Current teamPanther RacingCar number44Former teamsCheever RacingRahal Letterman RacingDreyer & Reinbold RacingRubicon Racing TeamStarts97Wins3Poles5Best finish3rd in 2004Previous series20061998–2000, 20021996–1997Champ Car World SeriesToyota AtlanticU...
Roger MorrisUskup ColchesterMorris pada 2014GerejaGereja InggrisKeuskupanKeuskupan ChelmsfordMasa jabatan25 Juli 2014–kiniPendahuluChristopher MorganJabatan lainDeakon Agung Worcester (2008–2014)ImamatTahbisan imam1994Tahbisan uskup25 Juli 2014Informasi pribadiLahir18 Juli 1968 (umur 55)KewarganegaraanBritania RayaDenominasiAnglikanPasangan hidupSallyAnak2 putriPendidikanImperial College, LondonTrinity College, Cambridge Roger Anthony Brett Morris (lahir 18 Juli 1968) adalah Uskup Co...
Kamboja Artikel ini adalah bagian dari seri Politik dan KetatanegaraanKerajaan Kamboja Monarki Raja Norodom Sihamoni Dewan Takhta Wangsa Norodom Wangsa Sisowath (1904–41) Pemerintahan Perdana Menteri (daftar) Hun Sen Wakil Perdana Menteri Sar Kheng Tea Banh Hor Namhong Men Sam An Bin Chhin Yim Chhaily Kong Sam Ol Kabinet Parlemen Senat Presiden: Say Chhum Majelis Nasional Presiden: Heng Samrin Pemilihan umum Komite Pemilihan Umum Nasional Pemilihan umum terakhir Majelis Nasional: 20132018 S...
Angelou tampil dengan puisinya, On the Pulse of Morning saat pelantikan pertama Bill Clinton sebagai Presiden Amerika Serikat, pada tahun 1993 On the Pulse of Morning adalah puisi karya penulis dan penyair berkebangsaan Amerika Serikat, Maya Angelou yang dibacakan pada perhelatan pelantikan pertama Bill Clinton sebagai Presiden Amerika Serikat, 20 Januari 1993. Melalui pembacaan puisinya di muka umum, Angelou menjadi penyair kedua dalam sejarah Amerika Serikat yang membacakan puisi dalam pela...
Belgian basketball player Tomas Van Den SpiegelVan den Spiegel in a warm up with Real Madrid.Personal informationBorn (1978-07-10) 10 July 1978 (age 45)Ghent, BelgiumNationalityBelgianListed height7 ft 0.25 in (2.14 m)Listed weight260 lb (118 kg)Career informationNBA draft2000: undraftedPlaying career1995–2012PositionCenterCareer history1995–1997Okapi Aalstar1997–2001Telindus Oostende2001–2002Fortitudo Bologna2002Telindus Oostende2002–2004Fortitudo Bolo...
Season of television series Big Brother 18Hosted byJulie ChenNo. of days99No. of houseguests16WinnerNicole FranzelRunner-upPaul AbrahamianAmerica's Favorite HouseguestVictor ArroyoCompanion shows Big Brother: Live Chat Big Brother: After Dark Country of originUnited StatesNo. of episodes42ReleaseOriginal networkCBSOriginal releaseJune 22 (2016-06-22) –September 21, 2016 (2016-09-21)Additional informationFilming datesJune 15 (15-06) –September 21, 2016 (2016-0...
إنجلترا في العصور الوسطى المتأخرة هي فترة من تاريخ انجلترا في فترة العصور الوسطى المتأخرة، والممتدة منذ القرن الثالث عشر حينما انتهى عصر ملوك إنجلترا الأنجويين وتبوأ هنري الثالث العرش –والذي يعتبره الكثيرون بداية سلالة بلانتاجينت التي حكمت إنجلترا– وحتى تولي سلالة تود...
71. GroßmeisterFerdinand von Hompesch zu BolheimGroßmeisterwappen von Ferdinand von Hompesch zu BolheimSchloss Bollheim um 1860, Sammlung Alexander Duncker, 1882 abgerissenFamilienwappen derer von HompeschMünze aus dem Jahr der Vertreibung von Malta Ferdinand Joseph Hermann Anton Freiherr von Hompesch zu Bolheim (* 9. November 1744 auf Schloss Bollheim bei Oberelvenich; † 12. Mai 1805 in Montpellier, Frankreich) war der 71. und bisher einzige deutsche Großmeister des Souveränen Maltese...